声音影响饮食行为:实证进展与理论构思
作者: 余习德 张小娟 / 13091次阅读 时间: 2017年5月13日
来源: 《心理科学进展》
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网参考文献

E\Q R C6@H0

LfP1y4W^ Xt0崔丽娟 , 张高产. (2004). 内隐联结测验 (IAT)研究回顾与展望. 心理科学, 27, 161–164. 心理学空间m2?1{]o4ci$t e

心理学空间#y$a2PMH tz

黄宇鸿. (2007). 释"和""谐". 广西社会科学 , (5), 135–137.

w dE#O[pdV0

K7n`T+CK&J Y7s~/E0王远坤. (2005). “声味相和 ”,一种有意味的形式 ——论中国歌舞艺术与饮食. 饮食文化研究, (3), 104–112.

znp:`F6X0心理学空间)__ w$K4z@"N_Ma-[

叶浩生. (2014). “具身”涵义的理论辨析 . 心理学报 , 46, 1032–1042. 心理学空间)P2vI&A N+K.s-|

E4X`#L_0叶浩生. (2010). 具身认知 : 认知心理学的新取向 . 心理科学进展, 18, 705–710. 心理学空间6GN(N-QO$?r@

*nAur#pu:b/J%w0叶浩生. (2011). 有关具身认知思潮的理论心理学思考 . 心理学报 , 43, 589–598. 心理学空间^/wD)Jg } Lj

心理学空间 nZ(Y6C%I1D

周雯, 冯果. (2012).嗅知觉及其与情绪系统的交互 . 心理科学进展, 20, 2–9.

\;[V5r*e0心理学空间`Z ~ yZ"t'x

Areni, C. S., & Kim, D. (1993). The influence of background music on shopping behavior: classical versus top-forty music in a wine store. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 336–340. 心理学空间'Lr NVQ-NX(i

心理学空间'M(HGn D2c

Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 11818–11823. 心理学空间"G ws S2@+}E

fC(v }({0Bronner K., Frieler, K., Bruhn, H., Hirt, R., & Piper, D. (2012). What is the sound of citrus? Research on the correspondences between the perception of sound and flavour. In C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatis, K. Pastiadis (Eds.), Proceedings of the ICMPC, ESCOM 2012 joint conference. Thessaloniki, Greece: ICMPC. 心理学空间+ipz!Aj

9k"k%eb P.|H*[?)^?(k0Brown, R. L. (1958). Wrapper influence on the perception of freshness in bread. Journal of Applied Psychology, 42, 257–260.

n^ o(O5N0

W!E W#e&\#R0Budinger, E., Heil, P., Hess, A., & Scheich, H. (2006). Multisensory processing via early cortical stages: Connections of the primary auditory cortical field with other sensory systems. Neuroscience, 143, 1065–1083.

4wBZ t~0

C,QP-A2JM0Budinger, E., Laszcz, A., Lison, H., Scheich, H., & Ohl, F. W. (2008). Non-sensory cortical and subcortical connections of the primary auditory cortex in Mongolian gerbils: Bottom-up and top-down processing of neuronal information via field AI. Brain Research, 1220, 2–32. 心理学空间E3M`4u%L$sBn

心理学空间L }!d])[o0qr8U4n

Calvert, G. A., Spence, C., & Stein, B. E. (2004). The handbook of multisensory processes. Cambridge, MA, UK: MIT Press.

;g:R p2_-p q)?;c!]0

*I4J y_5NWBW0Carvalho, F. R., van Ee, R., Rychtarikova, M., Touhafi, A., Steenhaut, K., & Persoone, D., … Leman, M. (2015). Does music influence the multisensory tasting experience?. Journal of Sensory Studies, 30, 404–412.

twE D8e3n0心理学空间Vx"A+P0y2R

Carvalho, F. R., Wang, Q., van Ee, R., & Spence, C. (2016). The influence of soundscapes on the perception and evaluation of beers. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 32– 心理学空间y5@*i8B4q [/_

#lWf)f ts0|041.

dm%F+bj gJ|0

%`6Z2MIU'FE7f(h*vy0Christensen, C. M., & Vickers, Z. M. (1981). Relationships of chewing sounds to judgments of food crispness. Journal of Food Science, 46, 574–578. 心理学空间qFGh@r/hfW,q"{

心理学空间O`)h,X#B4u

Crisinel, A. S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J., & Spence, C. (2012). A bittersweet symphony: Systematically modulating the taste of food by changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 201–204.

J cT%I+_.yED[0

4C;pd@nZM0Crisinel, A. S., Jacquier, C., Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Composing with cross-modal correspondences: Music and odors in concert. Chemosensory Perception, 6, 45–52. 心理学空间3F&dMeY;~x]

)j9t lPP[ \0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters, 464, 39–42. 心理学空间2p;Y IWq

心理学空间K zh/A | Nt_

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010a). A sweet sound? Food names reveal implicit associations between taste and pitch. Perception, 39, 417–425.

9_T L+A'sM:}cO1p0心理学空间B4a@J7z_.U|

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010b). As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1994–2002.

*V$[AE0sw L0

UR&tx"I)nY o&N&U%U0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal associations between flavoured milk solutions and musical notes. Acta Psychologica, 138, 155–161.

e^+_3Nk+|&wDU4y0心理学空间'N)aA3e*dS!mh gz e_

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012a). A Fruity Note: Crossmodal associations between odors and musical notes. Chemical Senses, 37, 151–158.

j+nj!T&[|,s4Q,{4D0心理学空间JN$vu(U

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012b). The impact of pleasantness ratings on crossmodal associations between food samples and musical notes. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 136–140.

-K Z4Z[ v Hi6K `0心理学空间)K ZB&I"St{\

Cummings, A., .eponien., R., Koyama, A., Saygin, A. P., Townsend, J., & Dick, F. (2006). Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds. Brain Research, 1115, 92–107.

z,\Z aW x(Q(xN0

0L&l z4m'^ PK \ge$W5F0Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. The Making of Consciousness Harcourt Brace, 2, 81–91.

r fNY5U0心理学空间hut#e-|

Deadwyler, S. A., Foster, T. C., & Hampson, R. E. (1987). Processing of sensory information in the hippocampus. CRC Critical Reviews in Clinical Neurobiology, 2, 335. 心理学空间4d{dK9c'D

心理学空间9sq1U;i"i$Cn

Delwiche, J. F. (2003). Attributes believed to impact flavor: An opinion survey. Journal of Sensory Studies, 18, 347–352.

3kV(S%Pp7l$X0心理学空间v'P1E#M*H[e

Demattè, M. L., Pojer, N., Endrizzi, I., Corollaro, M. L., Betta, M., Aprea, E., …Gasperi, F. (2014). Effects of the sound of the bite on apple perceived crispness and hardness. Food Quality and Preference, 38, 58–64.

&I%Z\/ZP+yc,t&v0

g;g?oR9_)`O0Dijksterhuis, G., Boucon, C., & Le Berre, E. (2014). Increasing saltiness perception through perceptual constancy created by expectation. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 24–28. 心理学空间*nwy&G-fj'H

心理学空间!Y'F(g"e{

Duizer, L. (2001). A review of acoustic research for studying the sensory perception of crisp, crunchy and crackly textures. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 12, 17–24.

%DEv4vWt2yU0

6e mj1{ ]3f O;p9F0Ferber, C., & Cabanac, M. (1987). Influence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet or salt. Appetite, 8, 229–235. 心理学空间If4i g4LK

Mpo_.tX$VrdE0Fiegel, A., Meullenet, J. F., Harrington, R. J., Humble, R., & Seo, H. S. (2014). Background music genre can modulate flavor pleasantness and overall impression of food stimuli. Appetite, 76, 144–152.

T/QKW;}3fo e!a0

M } XM{0Forsyth, A., & Cloonan, M. (2008). Alco-pop? The use of popular music in Glasgow pubs. Popular Music and Society, 31, 57–78.

"Qv{%n1U0

n cK l%_%F2@0Fusar-Poli, P., & Stanghellini, G. (2009). Maurice merleau-ponty and the "embodied subjectivity" (1908–61). Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 23, 91–93. 心理学空间[di5K6yL*[.V:}

3xWC%v)C S.Z$^K8Pn0Galindo, M. M., Schneider, N. Y., St.hler, F., T.le, J., & Meyerhof, W. (2012). Taste preferences. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 108, 383–

1Q8ji3W)Vi.{{8d0

9S l;_.k2l2MO0426.

X/t?gf sG]0

5hQk3Q k'@0Grabenhorst, F., & Rolls, E. T. (2008). Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 723–729.

0uV+Q+NQb }%o0

*u%P*dRr'~ ~}+_'SZ0Guéguen, N., Jacob, C., Le Guellec, H., Morineau, T., & Lourel, M. (2008). Sound level of environmental music and drinking behavior: A field experiment with beer drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 1795–1798.

Na.\'\G0n0HC0心理学空间AGh5js:z

Guéguen, N., Le Guellec, H., & Jacob, C. (2004). Sound level of background music and alcohol consumption: an empirical evaluation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 34–

Y E5`oZf#I0心理学空间Z"u C2gBH/Ww

38.

o)iE?ih&[0心理学空间$GoJ`*Lp

Ikemoto, S. (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: Two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens–olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Research Reviews, 56, 27–78.

!KJ5@/My5AJ/l0

.@/[kvf8b f0Kejr, A., Gigante, C., Hames, V., Krieg, C., Mages, J., K.nig, N., … Diel F. (2010). Receptive music therapy and salivary histamine secretion. Inflammation Research, 59, 217–218.

w"lF\ ~5E \k/e0

2G'~(DX3UgP0Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language and meaning: Brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 302–307.

e?0g_$o0

aT8x8Pq#xG0Koelsch, S. (2012). Brain and music. Oxford, England: John

]{FnVC ?0心理学空间[.a'EEj9m2uk

Wiley & Sons. 心理学空间Qo4qA(R[u

心理学空间B eX fu

Kontukoski, M., Luomala, H., Mesz, B., Sigman, M., Trevisan, M., Rotola-Pukkila, M., & Hopia A. I. (2015). Sweet and sour: Music and taste associations. Nutrition & Food Science, 45, 357–376. 心理学空间I;h B.f iR R

_L!\ uo0Kn.ferle, K. (2012). Using customer insights to improve product sound design. Marketing Review St. Gallen, 29, 47–53.

r8C].sZ(q m s0心理学空间)UT3w;WHR&Dd{

Kn.ferle, K., & Spence, C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 992–1006.

:^"Je6fP0

+@#O#GGB*v.B&YS0Kn.ferle, K. M., Woods, A., K.ppler, F., & Spence, C. (2015). That sounds sweet: Using cross-modal correspondences to communicate gustatory attributes. Psychology & Marketing, 32, 107–120. 心理学空间2rN.a%SeG

心理学空间O Cyk;SXe ]

Kupfkrmann, I. (1964). Eating behaviour induced by sounds. Nature, 201, 324.

.Yt8{? kqia(Wf2Z0

1CO(v5B9L;_0Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 819–831. 心理学空间/x:vk`7O

uh oRE0[0Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

mv8~2tq PF0

c?l"r^]$d'n i0McCarron, A., & Tierney, K. J. (1989). The effect of auditory stimulation on the consumption of soft drinks. Appetite, 13, 155–159.

g,[E!\O&E X/x0心理学空间6H"S%g&`XZN#?

Mesz, B., Sigman, M., & Trevisan, M. A. (2012). A composition algorithm based on crossmodal taste-music correspondences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 71.

6SA jr5Nx)s6g0心理学空间ua$uLRB$U

Mesz, B., Trevisan, M. A., & Sigman, M. (2011). The taste of music. Perception, 40, 209–219. 心理学空间e ~'M:~P`2h

x%}+D2C(Q&a I2S0Milliman, R. E. (1986). The influence of background music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 286–289. 心理学空间0ga8^ kqj~

心理学空间s K'`#Qn[

Newquist, G., & Gardner, R. A. (2015). Reconsidering food reward, brain stimulation, and dopamine: Incentives act forward. The American Journal of Psychology, 128, 431–444. 心理学空间%_jY(Ipi:},B-X-T4Ua

#VPD5Q$o%k0North, A. C. (2012). The effect of background music on the taste of wine. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 293–

l2`-n$zT%U#l0心理学空间2UEJ+O b iD\

301. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & Mckendrick, J. (1997). In-store music affects product choice. Nature, 390, 132. 心理学空间3S}&^u N.[4s|)g"D/N

0d \M B2U(@OO%E,a ]M0North, A. C., Sheridan, L. P., & Areni, C. S. (2016). Music congruity effects on product memory, perception, and choice. Journal of Retailing, 92, 83–95.

z?9j{UQ0]0

/Jymjz"} w!w0Patel, A. D., & Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: The action simulation for auditory prediction (ASAP) hypothesis.

+j#v^ak)e k0心理学空间#QI }C$M3LxP0e

心理学空间}9V KG},] O }T

心理学空间9t CsTROD Z

Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 57. 心理学空间t'\YX^T F

心理学空间fr~ @Q'@!s+U6H

Plailly, J., Howard, J. D., Gitelman, D. R., & Gottfried, J. A. (2008). Attention to odor modulates thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 5257–5267.

7Pn:M?O Rz$h0心理学空间 r4hr+Bq,S

Plotto, A., Margaría, C. A., Goodner, K. L., Goodrich, R., & Baldwin, E. A. (2004). Odour and flavour thresholds for key aroma components in an orange juice matrix: Terpenes and aldehydes. Flavour and Fragrance Journal, 19, 491–498.

(TX;hja%K#Z0心理学空间"P-{H:t&gW;\*j[

Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 284–294.

C{9I6~U5Z"M0心理学空间8Ic ~7ao

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 257–262. 心理学空间~%j ]\%xmM,xT:b

7QlBrX v;\N0Seo, H. S., H.hner, A., Gudziol, V., Scheibe, M., & Hummel, 心理学空间-w'@ S!_E \:ui`5B

8HI2l0@"sz.Az0T. (2012). Influence of background noise on the performance in the odor sensitivity task: Effects of noise type and extraversion. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 89–97. 心理学空间_[(|U'VPF2~(H

心理学空间P;iu8F$[1d`X

Seo, H. S., & Hummel, T. (2011). Auditory–olfactory integration: Congruent or pleasant sounds amplify odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 36, 301–309.

9fpY7w\|eDE0E+pv0

a|!G.CS0Seo, H. S., Lohse, F., Luckett, C. R., & Hummel, T. (2014). Congruent sound can modulate odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 39, 215–228.

_0Xjs$j/VS0心理学空间 _(f ?9cQ&r H

Slocombe, B. G., Carmichael, D. A., & Simner, J. (2016). Cross-modal tactile–taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia, 88, 58–64.

b%Nvm NTeP#i0

(v{.K&Xs)p |~0Spence, C. (2012). Auditory contributions to flavour perception and feeding behaviour. Physiology & Behavior, 107, 505–515.

\O-A&Xpe0心理学空间Nw#D~*Pd({5a:c

Spence, C. (2015a). Eating with our ears: Assessing the importance of the sounds of consumption on our perception and enjoyment of multisensory flavour experiences. Flavour, 4, 3. 心理学空间,a%L6asfJ

8Ezj$w$|*{s:k^d0Spence, C. (2015b). Multisensory flavor perception. Cell, 161, 24–35.

4aj/D^RLSo0心理学空间9l Rk#v c2jt

Spence, C. (2011). Mouth-watering: The influence of environmental and cognitive factors on salivation and gustatory/flavor perception. Journal of Texture Studies, 42, 157–171. 心理学空间 ^9Zz(b9Qo8@ w?

心理学空间6P)J XHA2u0L a

Spence, C. (2014). Noise and its impact on the perception of food and drink. Flavour, 3, 9. 心理学空间@P"~NrO w!D

2~2c/e\*W}V0Spence, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2015). The perfect meal: The multisensory science of food and dining. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

FQ ICL-T [0

"L`@ i9}$a(T!w0Spence, C., & Shankar, M. U. (2010). The influence of auditory cues on the perception of, and responses to, food and drink. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 406–430. 心理学空间N&n/l x1F

i5kdg)I$D)ps*N0Spence, C., & Wang, Q. (2015). Sensory expectations elicited by the sounds of opening the packaging and pouring a beverage. Flavour, 4, 35.

4Zz4Q CZze0心理学空间6R0Nr'J-a:knz `b$]

Spence, C., & Zampini, M. (2006). Auditory contributions to multisensory product perception. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 92, 1009–1025.

E$L5X'| t%v$}@0L0心理学空间L7[0t-g3M Y$x Ur

Stafford, L. D., Fernandes, M., & Agobiani, E. (2012). Effects of noise and distraction on alcohol perception. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 218–224.

@ CW i@0

,cH/wPf^,P N*q0Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press.

4?.QPt W$JY.oi0心理学空间Ic0|vF7jB

Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Affective priming effects of musical sounds on the processing of word meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 604– 心理学空间.y'H$^e2eh$?3e F{ [o

D{9z.\Y*d LDKi]0621. Stevenson, R. J. (2012). The role of attention in flavour perception. Flavour, 1, 2. Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2004). Effect of ambience on food intake and food choice. Nutrition, 20, 821–838.

2uZM4h#xuZ:j0心理学空间8o ~%y,w/s9Z w d

Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2006). Listening to music while eating is related to increases in people's food intake and meal duration. Appetite, 47, 285–289. 心理学空间3`3AU5_O{ JF

心理学空间4}b5CtW

van Aken, G. A. (2013). Acoustic emission measurement of rubbing and tapping contacts of skin and tongue surfaces in relation to tactile perception. Food Hydrocolloids, 31, 325–331.

NNo gL;_0心理学空间{%W U$d]BX8}B

van Steenberghe, D., van der Glas, H. W., Grisar, P. R., & Vande Putte, K. M. (1981). The effect of acoustic masking on the silent period in the masseter electromyogram in man during sustained isometric contraction. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 21, 611–625.

#_+_0l_Z0

"N3Oq"lx1e&v0Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., & Spence, C. (2014). Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 88–95.

D'pc0p ?y0心理学空间{X } Ct

Walker, P. (2012). Cross-sensory correspondences and cross talk between dimensions of connotative meaning: visual angularity is hard, high-pitched, and bright. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1792–1809. 心理学空间|%Gn MWg)@

心理学空间Q"H|(h|A H,pZQ

Wang, Q., & Spence, C. (2016). ‘Striking a sour note’: Assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception. Multisensory Research, 29, 195–208.

FKd M;l#r.tD;T0心理学空间8g iPu)|Y-Y

Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015a). Assessing the effect of musical congruency on wine tasting in a live performance setting. i-Perception, 6, 2041669515593027. 心理学空间w0j8k.d*CZ \]&W _;n

心理学空间kX|'~-H!}

Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015b). Assessing the influence of the multisensory atmosphere on the taste of vodka. Beverages, 1, 204–217.

PqU*?H!fNZ4w0

Jepox|0Wesson, D. W., & Wilson, D. A. (2010). Smelling sounds: Olfactory-auditory sensory convergence in the olfactory 心理学空间t4U$jX"_,xW:?

心理学空间(l2F*?Nn

B5LhN+w.Do0

'cu]$j/t$~!FJ0tubercle. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3013–3021. Yang, S. J., Gallo, D. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Embodied

5N/{,K WwX'd;m|7h0

:j[ l*n;t(P+y0Woods, A. T., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., Poliakoff, E., memory judgments: A case of motor fluency. Journal of Dijksterhuis, G. B., & Thomas, A. (2011). Expected taste Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and intensity affects response to sweet drinks in primary taste Cognition, 35, 1359–1365. cortex. Neuroreport, 22, 365–369. Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues 心理学空间:L4MJ[a[g#LLm

心理学空间^*vpeq5m

Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Dijksterhuis, G. B., in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of & Thomas, A. (2010). Flavor expectation: The effect of potato chips. Journal of Sensory Studies, 19, 347–363. assuming homogeneity on drink perception. Chemosensory Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2005). Modifying the Perception, 3, 174–181. multisensory perception of a carbonated beverage using

Y4fB'v;ZL)qC%XG@0

]^RZ2vXC9K0Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., auditory cues. Food Quality and Preference, 16, 632–641. Hodson, R., Gonda, H., … Thomas, A. (2011). Effect of Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: background noise on food perception. Food Quality and Does social exclusion literally feel cold?. Psychological Preference, 22, 42–47. Science, 19, 838–842. 心理学空间F(DN7@`

-~'M`1FFc C0Sound influence eating or drinking behavior: Empirical progress and theoretical ideas 心理学空间,x e9nclgu

Z\y"{gn,s ET }3}r0YU Xide1; ZHANG Xiaojuan2; LU Cheng3; ZHU Yiyi1; GAO Dingguo1 心理学空间PA+T8n.GU|Y:]0W

心理学空间S*MK Ngp3?

(1 Department of psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of

XOo$D"[ ~0心理学空间bMF4Y DUH

Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Guangzhou 510006, China) 心理学空间 f u Q W#GO

心理学空间|%Sn?TO

(2 Chaoyang Experimental School, Shantou 515100, China)

-_0Zn:a`|+o0

w`7O9eNh~/Ph1U0(3 Nanfang College of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou 510970, China) 心理学空间 \k3f-v7{-c2K

心理学空间7~j.J ['| }ncF)^

Abstract: Dietary behaviors consist of a series of psychological and behavior processes, such as sensing flavors, evaluating food’s and drink’s qualities or hedonic valuation, forming preferences for foods and drinks, as well as the explicit consuming actions and patterns. Dietary behaviors are influenced by sound, which, specifically, affects the sensitivity of the gustatory system and individuals’ food preferences. Sounds contain inter-oceptive cues and exter-oceptive cues that directly affect dietary behaviors. Inter-oceptive cues refer to sounds created when one interacts with foods, such as chewing sound, swallowing sound and food preparation sound etc.. Exter-oceptive cues refer to environmental or background sounds, such as music played in a restaurant. Studies have shown that the effects of sound on dietary behaviors are essentially cognitive, involving, for example attention, sensory integration (the match effect), and familiarity and expectation (the potential tone painting effect). The interaction between sound and dietary behaviors has also been studied by neuroscientists, who aim to uncover the underlying mechanisms, and from the embodied cognitive perspective. Both are expected to provide insights about dietary behaviors. Key words: noise; interactive sound; background music; dietary behavior; across-modal integration

3rN @6x GB0www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
«自尊的认知神经机制 人格与社会心理学
《人格与社会心理学》
第18章 人类情绪的发生»