声音影响饮食行为:实证进展与理论构思
作者: 余习德 张小娟 / 13328次阅读 时间: 2017年5月13日
来源: 《心理科学进展》
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网参考文献心理学空间;^*XJ n `&iw.n

心理学空间3`WemL~.B7D Y

崔丽娟 , 张高产. (2004). 内隐联结测验 (IAT)研究回顾与展望. 心理科学, 27, 161–164. 心理学空间vG l/pZ!xTX4F

-c_'x6F~&dCvA;_0黄宇鸿. (2007). 释"和""谐". 广西社会科学 , (5), 135–137.

_'{/o!a [Y0心理学空间W"`[$k)h+z pL5n

王远坤. (2005). “声味相和 ”,一种有意味的形式 ——论中国歌舞艺术与饮食. 饮食文化研究, (3), 104–112.

{3K!r#zY6c-Ab5?0

(ZvW3ZLSq%L%A z/d0叶浩生. (2014). “具身”涵义的理论辨析 . 心理学报 , 46, 1032–1042. 心理学空间1Iq,r]8H.U$H

心理学空间0Z4HZt/La q)n~

叶浩生. (2010). 具身认知 : 认知心理学的新取向 . 心理科学进展, 18, 705–710. 心理学空间9N[Z?V(i#S

心理学空间:}MD"?Olo

叶浩生. (2011). 有关具身认知思潮的理论心理学思考 . 心理学报 , 43, 589–598. 心理学空间 vg-O8`ZM1o

or/YU0a4Xx0周雯, 冯果. (2012).嗅知觉及其与情绪系统的交互 . 心理科学进展, 20, 2–9.

ez BzMy0心理学空间Mq)\+^6E [,Md#v K

Areni, C. S., & Kim, D. (1993). The influence of background music on shopping behavior: classical versus top-forty music in a wine store. Advances in Consumer Research, 20, 336–340.

T$K6p'f a4?0心理学空间(mfA6]6B6i^

Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 11818–11823.

&v:oJ)B\ap0心理学空间n4U*g(C4xZm3d

Bronner K., Frieler, K., Bruhn, H., Hirt, R., & Piper, D. (2012). What is the sound of citrus? Research on the correspondences between the perception of sound and flavour. In C. Tsougras, P. Mavromatis, K. Pastiadis (Eds.), Proceedings of the ICMPC, ESCOM 2012 joint conference. Thessaloniki, Greece: ICMPC. 心理学空间KLI O)X{

_$V]QM7m\;YDL0Brown, R. L. (1958). Wrapper influence on the perception of freshness in bread. Journal of Applied Psychology, 42, 257–260. 心理学空间}1UUH6J+I

u fAk5l^V0Budinger, E., Heil, P., Hess, A., & Scheich, H. (2006). Multisensory processing via early cortical stages: Connections of the primary auditory cortical field with other sensory systems. Neuroscience, 143, 1065–1083.

Z,z@4AO1q [0

pkF$RK5xv`0Budinger, E., Laszcz, A., Lison, H., Scheich, H., & Ohl, F. W. (2008). Non-sensory cortical and subcortical connections of the primary auditory cortex in Mongolian gerbils: Bottom-up and top-down processing of neuronal information via field AI. Brain Research, 1220, 2–32.

%cG F})sp8\ X&M0心理学空间#p?'O*Sj)W#hw:?8o

Calvert, G. A., Spence, C., & Stein, B. E. (2004). The handbook of multisensory processes. Cambridge, MA, UK: MIT Press. 心理学空间0q|tGU$Du(mCo#R

心理学空间VnCw-f4[;[

Carvalho, F. R., van Ee, R., Rychtarikova, M., Touhafi, A., Steenhaut, K., & Persoone, D., … Leman, M. (2015). Does music influence the multisensory tasting experience?. Journal of Sensory Studies, 30, 404–412.

3P,a A2h+W.Nu#@"~sg0心理学空间\5Y9S_G

Carvalho, F. R., Wang, Q., van Ee, R., & Spence, C. (2016). The influence of soundscapes on the perception and evaluation of beers. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 32– 心理学空间`V9b!D7J!jD

0K\ @"jHr&~041.

v:\"R+I)H4E_4BH2l_0

4pL^Le:Ys3p6f0Christensen, C. M., & Vickers, Z. M. (1981). Relationships of chewing sounds to judgments of food crispness. Journal of Food Science, 46, 574–578. 心理学空间:@6k#@;n-q Y4I6L

心理学空间qCCM#TF\iE

Crisinel, A. S., Cosser, S., King, S., Jones, R., Petrie, J., & Spence, C. (2012). A bittersweet symphony: Systematically modulating the taste of food by changing the sonic properties of the soundtrack playing in the background. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 201–204. 心理学空间6G8b^ ~%u^m

心理学空间~(n0rHz)Ji"x

Crisinel, A. S., Jacquier, C., Deroy, O., & Spence, C. (2013). Composing with cross-modal correspondences: Music and odors in concert. Chemosensory Perception, 6, 45–52.

3l/[+g dJ}9r @(J0心理学空间1c'y8m0Yw

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2009). Implicit association between basic tastes and pitch. Neuroscience Letters, 464, 39–42. 心理学空间Uq'L1t#X

心理学空间}c,`V M&[

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010a). A sweet sound? Food names reveal implicit associations between taste and pitch. Perception, 39, 417–425. 心理学空间 ~P ?*V!`

$o9j2Mr:mh{w/P0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2010b). As bitter as a trombone: Synesthetic correspondences in nonsynesthetes between tastes/flavors and musical notes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 1994–2002. 心理学空间,? _]W:sQ

心理学空间6A,G xu m/k^%_

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2011). Crossmodal associations between flavoured milk solutions and musical notes. Acta Psychologica, 138, 155–161. 心理学空间"o.T EL%um

2IT iJq6_L)E0Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012a). A Fruity Note: Crossmodal associations between odors and musical notes. Chemical Senses, 37, 151–158.

1i%b2vM"qBpa%c0心理学空间;C7`4mn0aL U,i

Crisinel, A. S., & Spence, C. (2012b). The impact of pleasantness ratings on crossmodal associations between food samples and musical notes. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 136–140. 心理学空间 axU q7@A

n@1Mb"n"R#BpK0Cummings, A., .eponien., R., Koyama, A., Saygin, A. P., Townsend, J., & Dick, F. (2006). Auditory semantic networks for words and natural sounds. Brain Research, 1115, 92–107. 心理学空间J"e4yj'C(l

心理学空间wS!A&aR

Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body, emotion and the making of consciousness. The Making of Consciousness Harcourt Brace, 2, 81–91. 心理学空间I hk(U,l `2ax

3^g/D}]8@?@0Deadwyler, S. A., Foster, T. C., & Hampson, R. E. (1987). Processing of sensory information in the hippocampus. CRC Critical Reviews in Clinical Neurobiology, 2, 335.

)Z3^)ys]v,lK0

G@2P So^.B0Delwiche, J. F. (2003). Attributes believed to impact flavor: An opinion survey. Journal of Sensory Studies, 18, 347–352.

m*JYH:UlpI0心理学空间.Y i;K0KU@:P$L2q3z

Demattè, M. L., Pojer, N., Endrizzi, I., Corollaro, M. L., Betta, M., Aprea, E., …Gasperi, F. (2014). Effects of the sound of the bite on apple perceived crispness and hardness. Food Quality and Preference, 38, 58–64.心理学空间f;a5LslLzM9^

心理学空间D ?&xC"c2j N

Dijksterhuis, G., Boucon, C., & Le Berre, E. (2014). Increasing saltiness perception through perceptual constancy created by expectation. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 24–28. 心理学空间V'P5`d C1E(S^K

[M Z3UT0Duizer, L. (2001). A review of acoustic research for studying the sensory perception of crisp, crunchy and crackly textures. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 12, 17–24. 心理学空间0m5n*tH D%D7tA

心理学空间9}m;D@D%]&C(A#~

Ferber, C., & Cabanac, M. (1987). Influence of noise on gustatory affective ratings and preference for sweet or salt. Appetite, 8, 229–235.

2\8IG o8}k[? K]0心理学空间%p0N4[,K_

Fiegel, A., Meullenet, J. F., Harrington, R. J., Humble, R., & Seo, H. S. (2014). Background music genre can modulate flavor pleasantness and overall impression of food stimuli. Appetite, 76, 144–152.

n.B FNW\2]0

6qw-Q6G:_0Forsyth, A., & Cloonan, M. (2008). Alco-pop? The use of popular music in Glasgow pubs. Popular Music and Society, 31, 57–78.

7O(A/T v*cw [R0心理学空间X S3R A3_`i)_U

Fusar-Poli, P., & Stanghellini, G. (2009). Maurice merleau-ponty and the "embodied subjectivity" (1908–61). Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 23, 91–93.

C/jWF2v,jd0

4j[8raRS0Galindo, M. M., Schneider, N. Y., St.hler, F., T.le, J., & Meyerhof, W. (2012). Taste preferences. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 108, 383– 心理学空间#B7n6D&\0_ZP]

|{2HPP.E0426.

3zW S*Va@t;r4S0

k:EV,g9rl0Grabenhorst, F., & Rolls, E. T. (2008). Selective attention to affective value alters how the brain processes taste stimuli. European Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 723–729. 心理学空间SW#G3deN&F

.z}#C%g s b0Guéguen, N., Jacob, C., Le Guellec, H., Morineau, T., & Lourel, M. (2008). Sound level of environmental music and drinking behavior: A field experiment with beer drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32, 1795–1798.

Ar;z;p*p?0

'_Z1Bd9|.Yb0Guéguen, N., Le Guellec, H., & Jacob, C. (2004). Sound level of background music and alcohol consumption: an empirical evaluation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 99, 34– 心理学空间R:\2UbU.y%Q!f XO

.}H*H}1t038. 心理学空间/P&Mnrv3I}(v A e

心理学空间i{n\o9}\}

Ikemoto, S. (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: Two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens–olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Research Reviews, 56, 27–78.

4_4rz3O5l ~dXh0心理学空间xo^#AL.W0f

Kejr, A., Gigante, C., Hames, V., Krieg, C., Mages, J., K.nig, N., … Diel F. (2010). Receptive music therapy and salivary histamine secretion. Inflammation Research, 59, 217–218. 心理学空间9d,\ y6CW'oa)wR

心理学空间.r `e*V0fl

Koelsch, S., Kasper, E., Sammler, D., Schulze, K., Gunter, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). Music, language and meaning: Brain signatures of semantic processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 302–307. 心理学空间 Z pO(I]v(mX

d6U(hrFt"d/wxOo0Koelsch, S. (2012). Brain and music. Oxford, England: John 心理学空间i3wdH`2M(~@

9T2Wr9p$v8R8h.T0Wiley & Sons.

'yTg.`9V\3SN0心理学空间^!mW|5e"f8v D*k

Kontukoski, M., Luomala, H., Mesz, B., Sigman, M., Trevisan, M., Rotola-Pukkila, M., & Hopia A. I. (2015). Sweet and sour: Music and taste associations. Nutrition & Food Science, 45, 357–376.

!E4s%nB;\x#LNx o0心理学空间8l2~-ie.T `*H L

Kn.ferle, K. (2012). Using customer insights to improve product sound design. Marketing Review St. Gallen, 29, 47–53. 心理学空间)n Dk0Jr

心理学空间-LO8aAO8Q6g+l

Kn.ferle, K., & Spence, C. (2012). Crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 992–1006. 心理学空间5Sx!Pot&C

a5s'K+p+t0Kn.ferle, K. M., Woods, A., K.ppler, F., & Spence, C. (2015). That sounds sweet: Using cross-modal correspondences to communicate gustatory attributes. Psychology & Marketing, 32, 107–120. 心理学空间,J2u(` v(lQ+qX

心理学空间mY$un/jL,~`

Kupfkrmann, I. (1964). Eating behaviour induced by sounds. Nature, 201, 324.

b5}0NHE k7A\{G pY0

0CJS6[Z0Labroo, A. A., Dhar, R., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Of frog wines and frowning watches: Semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 819–831.

Ry9d%T8c\+~p d0心理学空间9G5yu w.jz9V

Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books. 心理学空间+X._ P'v%?p4X

心理学空间mj3};kJ@ \{

McCarron, A., & Tierney, K. J. (1989). The effect of auditory stimulation on the consumption of soft drinks. Appetite, 13, 155–159.

w)b*Yk w\t}$n0

'p&\tYR*z0Mesz, B., Sigman, M., & Trevisan, M. A. (2012). A composition algorithm based on crossmodal taste-music correspondences. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 71.

Ik,{i v&JiM\:?)cq0

%Lv;go I0Mesz, B., Trevisan, M. A., & Sigman, M. (2011). The taste of music. Perception, 40, 209–219.

(Z!`2? rsY-E0k0心理学空间;i1j#iJ$N$e1P)O

Milliman, R. E. (1986). The influence of background music on the behavior of restaurant patrons. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 286–289. 心理学空间b(W YDp~

心理学空间,L{)CC6wmS5r)y:{

Newquist, G., & Gardner, R. A. (2015). Reconsidering food reward, brain stimulation, and dopamine: Incentives act forward. The American Journal of Psychology, 128, 431–444.

n"tF$aGmL0心理学空间SR,d)Z,L8AD

North, A. C. (2012). The effect of background music on the taste of wine. British Journal of Psychology, 103, 293–

1DoS iy+L4p[0

.D%pB$t8Ea0301. North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & Mckendrick, J. (1997). In-store music affects product choice. Nature, 390, 132.

6h6^'g C/|6Wx#|B0心理学空间)x#@HyYI

North, A. C., Sheridan, L. P., & Areni, C. S. (2016). Music congruity effects on product memory, perception, and choice. Journal of Retailing, 92, 83–95.

dT sC'Pv,{lU0心理学空间;i*|cZ1c,G:[6otZ

Patel, A. D., & Iversen, J. R. (2014). The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: The action simulation for auditory prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. 心理学空间 W(o%Q(\3O7L+IVI:R

心理学空间"Oazr~

心理学空间4Gq-P(_6V"h@%P.SY

心理学空间 E;O | J%Y\&Bc x

Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 57. 心理学空间hR,A$wn-tm%\

G|2^jr V0Plailly, J., Howard, J. D., Gitelman, D. R., & Gottfried, J. A. (2008). Attention to odor modulates thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 5257–5267.

/b/u.@1g:Dg)X6LZ0

c'i X/sAR2gi:{0Plotto, A., Margaría, C. A., Goodner, K. L., Goodrich, R., & Baldwin, E. A. (2004). Odour and flavour thresholds for key aroma components in an orange juice matrix: Terpenes and aldehydes. Flavour and Fragrance Journal, 19, 491–498. 心理学空间 o BBx F0WM)Y/B2_0b

PbYf Ro+v0Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 284–294.

YzDYE0M)F1Ov@Lfv `0心理学空间b'I0]`5k Q Q"mA$z

Salimpoor, V. N., Benovoy, M., Larcher, K., Dagher, A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2011). Anatomically distinct dopamine release during anticipation and experience of peak emotion to music. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 257–262.

k NS-F u$I0心理学空间 E8u3@D`

Seo, H. S., H.hner, A., Gudziol, V., Scheibe, M., & Hummel,

L2A-Nz!B V*N0心理学空间#_6?yG[ T0V L L4Kc

T. (2012). Influence of background noise on the performance in the odor sensitivity task: Effects of noise type and extraversion. Experimental Brain Research, 222, 89–97. 心理学空间F6t5AJA ^5~B`k

n2CAb+s4K0Seo, H. S., & Hummel, T. (2011). Auditory–olfactory integration: Congruent or pleasant sounds amplify odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 36, 301–309. 心理学空间H Rzh:iJ

心理学空间CJxb-XP

Seo, H. S., Lohse, F., Luckett, C. R., & Hummel, T. (2014). Congruent sound can modulate odor pleasantness. Chemical Senses, 39, 215–228.

Ej f^+J H0心理学空间5|d)Da,b&eXG(T0d

Slocombe, B. G., Carmichael, D. A., & Simner, J. (2016). Cross-modal tactile–taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia, 88, 58–64.

sYp~3R L0心理学空间vC.t]Rd4b!y1B2?

Spence, C. (2012). Auditory contributions to flavour perception and feeding behaviour. Physiology & Behavior, 107, 505–515.

&y b9O8Ni-b#Ku0心理学空间#YD8G"M-rn$x6C

Spence, C. (2015a). Eating with our ears: Assessing the importance of the sounds of consumption on our perception and enjoyment of multisensory flavour experiences. Flavour, 4, 3. 心理学空间3lP1~])AA n

(D3z;M5^T)h"D!H`0Spence, C. (2015b). Multisensory flavor perception. Cell, 161, 24–35.

gcM_ V!A Q0n"@0

-\kx {5\_UG(Y0Spence, C. (2011). Mouth-watering: The influence of environmental and cognitive factors on salivation and gustatory/flavor perception. Journal of Texture Studies, 42, 157–171.

.}CFS4{ C0心理学空间q&i)iWE|d.yv c

Spence, C. (2014). Noise and its impact on the perception of food and drink. Flavour, 3, 9.

&`L8wce;hV At$du,Zq0

|^Yk"h e Y0Spence, C., & Piqueras-Fiszman, B. (2015). The perfect meal: The multisensory science of food and dining. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 心理学空间-|1|] Xc:sH

心理学空间8Hn&T+d l0E!S/c

Spence, C., & Shankar, M. U. (2010). The influence of auditory cues on the perception of, and responses to, food and drink. Journal of Sensory Studies, 25, 406–430.

s`z2kLgz,J0

$Y K q"}%k uA:[K0Spence, C., & Wang, Q. (2015). Sensory expectations elicited by the sounds of opening the packaging and pouring a beverage. Flavour, 4, 35.

[Y r@F/s [g0

k2p*o {0C%Pp!O0Spence, C., & Zampini, M. (2006). Auditory contributions to multisensory product perception. Acta Acustica United with Acustica, 92, 1009–1025.

(e1b xP"O0心理学空间~G,ljQ/q-`.|wGE

Stafford, L. D., Fernandes, M., & Agobiani, E. (2012). Effects of noise and distraction on alcohol perception. Food Quality and Preference, 24, 218–224. 心理学空间)Y(Ysv)YGb

WR2BUr,g0AZR0Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press. 心理学空间Ok&}OEg1Z

心理学空间Dyg7Zc f]

Steinbeis, N., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Affective priming effects of musical sounds on the processing of word meaning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 604– 心理学空间0o.X\ S&z7L3]

*k6Wc;gtH/X&`8]0621. Stevenson, R. J. (2012). The role of attention in flavour perception. Flavour, 1, 2. Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2004). Effect of ambience on food intake and food choice. Nutrition, 20, 821–838. 心理学空间C(K `9~ ~sN}"\-m

心理学空间b,f:YcF cP(S

Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2006). Listening to music while eating is related to increases in people's food intake and meal duration. Appetite, 47, 285–289. 心理学空间gDNL*M0?:O

心理学空间&o,K9IKcH!Bs

van Aken, G. A. (2013). Acoustic emission measurement of rubbing and tapping contacts of skin and tongue surfaces in relation to tactile perception. Food Hydrocolloids, 31, 325–331. 心理学空间~'A%`2T F

p/mfd[3S+[F/]0van Steenberghe, D., van der Glas, H. W., Grisar, P. R., & Vande Putte, K. M. (1981). The effect of acoustic masking on the silent period in the masseter electromyogram in man during sustained isometric contraction. Electromyography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 21, 611–625.

E*tGRm;pT a.a5M0

.a~J7O2m j0Velasco, C., Salgado-Montejo, A., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., & Spence, C. (2014). Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Quality and Preference, 34, 88–95.

5Ka&nzT |Y0_0心理学空间Q"u&l*S+n!AJ V

Walker, P. (2012). Cross-sensory correspondences and cross talk between dimensions of connotative meaning: visual angularity is hard, high-pitched, and bright. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74, 1792–1809.

m~T'r8y0

W+W [Al*j0Wang, Q., & Spence, C. (2016). ‘Striking a sour note’: Assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception. Multisensory Research, 29, 195–208.

6_g_]D;~sV/D8Q0心理学空间M5e0hnv7i$w2N

Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015a). Assessing the effect of musical congruency on wine tasting in a live performance setting. i-Perception, 6, 2041669515593027. 心理学空间;dy;Gi P7N4jm

心理学空间V P#zefa

Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C. (2015b). Assessing the influence of the multisensory atmosphere on the taste of vodka. Beverages, 1, 204–217.

w^-BTv0

3C"I$P,c-A$@N0Wesson, D. W., & Wilson, D. A. (2010). Smelling sounds: Olfactory-auditory sensory convergence in the olfactory 心理学空间^B@"~/ZCI l9r

F%P9t7Awj#Z0

1EDyn4P)p0心理学空间u$R7~'Dy7~-fTB

tubercle. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 3013–3021. Yang, S. J., Gallo, D. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Embodied

jg-QRc1k%S0

6`G#zm`0b;uH0Woods, A. T., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., Poliakoff, E., memory judgments: A case of motor fluency. Journal of Dijksterhuis, G. B., & Thomas, A. (2011). Expected taste Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and intensity affects response to sweet drinks in primary taste Cognition, 35, 1359–1365. cortex. Neuroreport, 22, 365–369. Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2004). The role of auditory cues 心理学空间'vsd`7[1Q

(y9u0p O6t:_\0Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Dijksterhuis, G. B., in modulating the perceived crispness and staleness of & Thomas, A. (2010). Flavor expectation: The effect of potato chips. Journal of Sensory Studies, 19, 347–363. assuming homogeneity on drink perception. Chemosensory Zampini, M., & Spence, C. (2005). Modifying the Perception, 3, 174–181. multisensory perception of a carbonated beverage using

&OF/sI9Vt&@5W7Ku0心理学空间8Tp)Z/][ z#]KqO

Woods, A. T., Poliakoff, E., Lloyd, D. M., Kuenzel, J., auditory cues. Food Quality and Preference, 16, 632–641. Hodson, R., Gonda, H., … Thomas, A. (2011). Effect of Zhong, C. B., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2008). Cold and lonely: background noise on food perception. Food Quality and Does social exclusion literally feel cold?. Psychological Preference, 22, 42–47. Science, 19, 838–842. 心理学空间 B;| J6x7}8m4RY;] O

心理学空间0k#ljn/`oz

Sound influence eating or drinking behavior: Empirical progress and theoretical ideas 心理学空间REN+` g

心理学空间cV4L A C:R(F-?F

YU Xide1; ZHANG Xiaojuan2; LU Cheng3; ZHU Yiyi1; GAO Dingguo1

izM*JB$Qmy{0心理学空间.Q[C&`A&I7Rr

(1 Department of psychology, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangdong Provincial Key Laboratory of

$V3sNY%V @a }_0心理学空间'@T#ql @2S

Social Cognitive Neuroscience and Mental Health, Guangzhou 510006, China)

w5D@IV'\)Z+y0

0_9e0Os1wHb pL3?0(2 Chaoyang Experimental School, Shantou 515100, China)

lF2Rt{O(jX3}v0

t"Q@ j5];ur.Q0(3 Nanfang College of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou 510970, China)

B"u a!uJ:b0心理学空间h%O SY(a

Abstract: Dietary behaviors consist of a series of psychological and behavior processes, such as sensing flavors, evaluating food’s and drink’s qualities or hedonic valuation, forming preferences for foods and drinks, as well as the explicit consuming actions and patterns. Dietary behaviors are influenced by sound, which, specifically, affects the sensitivity of the gustatory system and individuals’ food preferences. Sounds contain inter-oceptive cues and exter-oceptive cues that directly affect dietary behaviors. Inter-oceptive cues refer to sounds created when one interacts with foods, such as chewing sound, swallowing sound and food preparation sound etc.. Exter-oceptive cues refer to environmental or background sounds, such as music played in a restaurant. Studies have shown that the effects of sound on dietary behaviors are essentially cognitive, involving, for example attention, sensory integration (the match effect), and familiarity and expectation (the potential tone painting effect). The interaction between sound and dietary behaviors has also been studied by neuroscientists, who aim to uncover the underlying mechanisms, and from the embodied cognitive perspective. Both are expected to provide insights about dietary behaviors. Key words: noise; interactive sound; background music; dietary behavior; across-modal integration 心理学空间?:eKe*r5T'_

www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
«自尊的认知神经机制 人格与社会心理学
《人格与社会心理学》
第18章 人类情绪的发生»