A Biographical Memoir of ROGER WILLIAM BROWN
作者: JEROME KAGAN / 8801次阅读 时间: 2015年12月23日
来源: 美国国家科学院 标签: 罗杰布朗
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

A Biographical Memoir of ROGER WILLIAM BROWN心理学空间'{M3l J$|'R
by JEROME KAGAN

p*Z"`R:e8w0心理学空间rl9c5{$QpI"`

n_x;q}zS5l6X0
Courtesy of Harvard University Archives 
ROGER WILLIAM BROWN 
April 14, 1925–December 11, 1997

i7Oq%E r!L,[t(UJ]0心理学空间'M4?Q;QJ+w]EY

心理学空间J4V!L5f U_&T4G o!B2g1C


|Vl-u e&[j0心理学空间qCcg.TE(M

#{$k5Q-bk@[0ROGER BROWN COMBINED a remarkably creative mind, natu-ral gentleness, and a passion for language into a per-sona marked by generosity, unfeigned humility, and a daz-zling writing talent. Although he was the father of develop-mental psycholinguistics—Roger’s students dominate this domain of inquiry—he moved away from the brightest spot on the stage to share credit with his students in order to promote their young careers. The title of his chair, “The John Lindsley Professor of Psychology in the memory of William James,” was especially apt for a psychologist who probed the subjective frame with a sureness that reflected his faith in the validity of personal experience as a primary source of evidence. There is the hint of a paradox in Roger’s wariness of Platonic abstractions that floated too far from their evidential origins. Roger loved language, but he dis-trusted words.

~&niruKqO0

6|!}J3C)?)Y jn+kW0It is certain that biographers are most likely to select his summary of the first stage of the acquisition of English as his seminal work, while simultaneously praising his ability to attract so many bright scholars to the study of language development. Yet, his first teaching assignment in 1952 was as a social psychologist at Harvard University. Roger had good reason to be proud of his 1986 text Social Psychology: The Second Edition. Although colleagues regarded it as the most original in the field, instructors did not know how to use a book that was punctuated so frequently with the author’s personal feelings—it broke too sharply with formulaic rules. The chapter on “Impressions of Personality,” for example, contains a section that only Roger could write. After telling the reader that he was on a spring break in Nassau, he adds, “I think of myself as extremely nonracist and would unhesitatingly check that position on a self-descriptive in-ventory. . . . At the University Health Service, if I am as-signed to a Pakistani woman physician with a diamond in her nostril, I do not flinch.”

0f(IoH&M*d"{Auw$l0心理学空间Wn&e*fse(u

The uncanny ability to devise experiments that probed phenomena everyone had experienced but no one investi-gated was one of the most colorful threads in the elegant tapestry of his career. Two of the most famous were the study of the exquisitely detailed memories each of us has of events that were accompanied by an acute, strong emotion, and the frustration that accompanies the inability to re-trieve a familiar word—often called “tip of the tongue.” Roger explored the former phenomenon with James Kulik by asking informants of different ethnic groups to remem-ber exactly where they were and what they were doing the moment they heard about some newsworthy event, like the assassinations of John F. Kennedy and Medgar Evers. Roger used the phrase “flash bulb memories” to convey the in-credible detail contained in these episodic retrievals by those for whom the event was emotionally relevant. He even specu-lated on the evolutionary advantage this competence con-ferred on our species.心理学空间p\%CG`oz {FF

心理学空间&aB ex%ib NV

With David McNeill, Roger asked students to guess the word they were searching for when they were in the “tip of the tongue” state. Many of these guesses were near misses that had the same first letter, same number of syllables, and same syllabic stress as the correct word, suggesting that nonsemantic features are stored as inherent components of every word. A person searching for the word “sextant,” for example, might guess secant, sextet, or sexton.心理学空间z(?!Fn-T(e.l;h b!o

心理学空间_j `;ph6Z:{2uK/}M S+m

Although his fans, students, and colleagues alike admired his discoveries and smiled at his graceful sentences, fewer have commented on the moral authority he brought to aca-demic settings. I watched Roger year after year listen qui-etly during a heated faculty discussion on a controversial issue, and then, at the perfect time, in a pace neither hur-ried nor hesitant, express his opinion. Usually his comment settled the issue as he had suggested. The reason for Roger’s extraordinary persuasiveness was that his colleagues had awarded him the position of representing the unselfish voice. All of us understood that his remarks were not motivated by self-aggrandizement, but by his notion of what was best for faculty, students, and university. This role is not an au-tomatic addition to the respect that follows fame, prizes, publications, and high intelligence. It is awarded—and rarely—by colleagues who have been pleasantly surprised by the fact that someone among them can be trusted.心理学空间&p+j0h4Q]#[|%mR Y*b

心理学空间/wgZ.m0x$iN o'DZF

Roger William Brown was born on April 14, 1925, in De-troit, Michigan, into a family of four brothers, together with two sons of the elder brother, that, by the time he was ready for school, began to suffer from the economic distress of the depression. Roger attended the Detroit public schools and, on graduation from high school, thought about be-coming a novelist of social protest. He enrolled in the Navy during his first year at the University of Michigan. He was accepted into the V-12 Program, went first to Oberlin Col-lege and then to midshipmen training at Columbia Univer-sity. Roger witnessed the battle of Okinawa and was on the first ship to enter Nagasaki harbor following the explosion of the atomic bomb. But there were many quiet days and nights at sea and much time to read. John B. Watson’s book Behaviorism, which had attracted other budding novelists to psychology—B. F. Skinner included—also tempted Roger to replace a career in writing with psychology. He earned his bachelor’s, master’s, and doctoral degrees at the Uni-versity of Michigan, the last in 1952. Although his doctoral research was on the social psychology of the authoritarian personality, a popular topic after the Second World War, the retention of a special curiosity about language rendered him receptive to chance events. He remembered—perhaps a flashbulb memory—a lecture on the phoneme by Charles Fries that fit his motivation perfectly. Uncertain about his talent as a novelist and discouraged by the complexity of the phenomena of social psychology, he discovered that an understanding of language appeared to possess the com-plexity and amenability to empirical inquiry that made it a perfect place to exploit his intellectual gifts and strongest passions.心理学空间7]#G.D"L-d"i

心理学空间$Fu(Yd0g*|)t

Roger joined the faculty of Harvard University as an in-structor in 1952 and was promoted to assistant professor the following year. He taught undergraduate courses in so-cial psychology and language and became a participant in a research group on cognitive processes led by Jerome Bruner. The concepts of artificial intelligence, language, and cogni-tion were replacing the antimentalist views of the behavior-ists, and there was a heady excitement over mind and lan-guage swirling around MIT and Harvard in the mid-1950s. Listen to Roger describe the ambiance:心理学空间T x2z8EgOC

心理学空间(ox T u#]` Le

Come with me, if you will, to Harvard and MIT in the early 1950s. Ameri-can linguistics is still structural; Noam Chomsky is a junior fellow at Harvard, and we are all unaware of the surprise he is preparing. Skinner’s behavior-ism is strong at Harvard, George Miller is still interested in communication theory, and there is a lot of excitement in Jerome Bruner’s cognition project about the work on concept formation that would be described in The Study of Thinking.

5V5JpxW9v!l ~y0

:k,c]3tQ5?;Un0In addition, Herbert Simon, Alan Newell, Marvin Minsky, and John McCarthy were applying the ideas of artificial intelligence to the study of mind. The new legitimacy of mind, thought, and language supported Roger’s intellec-tual interests, and he and Eric Lenneberg, in one of their first collaborations, tested the highly publicized view of Ben-jamin Lee Whorf that the language of a community must influence the way the speakers remember and reason. Al-though these two young scholars found some initial sup-port for that idea in a study of names for colors, four de-cades later Roger changed his mind following research by Brent Berlin and Paul Kay on the color words used by other cultures and a study by Eleanor Rosch-Heider on the Dani of New Guinea suggesting that some colors are naturally salient because of the brain’s construction, rather than be-cause of their names. The Dani showed good recognition memory for colors that their language did not name.心理学空间2I.wx b bL

-f ~;Jved6C0Roger wrote his first synthesis of the relations between language and mind during a sabbatical year in 1957. Words and Things was the first book on the psychology of language that was an obvious heir of this first stage of the cognitive revolution—and it remains in print forty years later. The ten chapters dealt with the nature of meaning, the relation between language and thought, and even the possibility that phonemes have symbolic connotations—for example, why most people feel that the word “ching” should mean light, while “chung” probably means heavy. Steven Pinker, one of Roger’s students, admitted that Words and Things was one of the inspirations for Pinker’s own book The Language In-stinct.

)Y F(EMt9K wR0心理学空间F bq4M Kd'pd!{

The year Roger wrote Words and Things also marked the year he moved to MIT to teach popular courses on social psychology and language. Roger returned to Harvard in 1962 with the title of professor of social psychology and a five-year grant from the National Institute of Mental Health to study the language acquisition of three children he and his students named Adam, Eve, and Sarah. The strategy of this study was Baconian in the extreme, for it consisted of the careful collection, transcription, and analysis of a large corpus of spontaneous conversations between each mother and her child in the naturalness of the home setting. It turned out that Roger was wise to gather extensive data on a few subjects rather than more superficial information from a larger group. The extensive protocols were discussed with a gifted group of young colleagues that included Ursula Bellugi, Colin Fraser, Courtney Cazden, Jean Berko Gleason, David McNeill, Dan Slobin, Sam Anderson, Richard Cromer, and Gordon Finley. A First Language: The Early Stages, pub-lished by Harvard University Press in 1973, generated a large number of provocative generalizations, even though it did not explain completely how children acquired lan-guage. Although everyone knew that children acquire lan-guage at different rates, the data revealed that these rates appear more uniform when each child’s attainment of a syntactic milestone is related to the average length of the child’s utterances; that is, the mean number of morphemes in a large number of statements. This measure—the “mean length of utterance”—has become a standard index of a child’s status in acquiring English.

/mS.xwz&?!Sm$Zq0

5{9fw%PD}O8F0Roger and his students also discovered that although chil-dren use abbreviated utterances like “dolly sit” or “hit cup,” their minds hold a complex proposition. The problem is that the young child cannot express the whole idea in lan-guage. Roger also detected remarkable uniformity in the order of acquisition of English morphemes. For example, the s inflection on verbs—come versus comes—is acquired later than the s inflection on nouns. Tag questions (He can play, can’t he?) emerge much later in development because these sentences require the grammatically complex processes of inversion, negation, and ellipsis. Most important for theory, the protocols revealed that parents do not praise sentences that are syntactically correct nor criticize those that are grammatically wrong. Rather, most parents react primarily to whether the child’s sentence is true or false. That discov-ery meant that Skinner’s contention, popular at the time, that grammar was learned in accord with the principles of reward and punishment had to be incorrect. Children learned the syntax of their language by relying on cognitive abili-ties, still poorly understood, with processes that remain a central puzzle in developmental psycholinguistics.心理学空间Exc'Kc%Mp^

H9] r%]*~_d VEj0Roger spent a sabbatical year—it turned out unsuccess-fully—trying to compose a summary of the later stages of language acquisition. The analyses of the older children’s speech did not yield insights with the power of those in-ferred from the first stage. Roger decided there was little value in publishing the idiosyncratic details contained in the grammars that required so many hours of work. Equally important, linguistic theory had begun to expand and Roger admitted to some reluctance over devoting the time required to learn the new formalisms. He once noted, “I have worked in different areas because I like beginnings, times when the curve of knowledge is rising steeply, when chunks of intel-lectual gold still lie on the surface to be discovered by who-ever looks first. When the incremental curve levels off and new discoveries become hard to make, I tend to look else-where.” Fortunately, his students, including Jill de Villiers, Helen Tager-Flusberg, and Kenji Hakuta, are now active in this domain.

L8OI%g Z;y1N*fx H0

n*B`8L2|o0Roger served as the last chairman of Harvard’s Depart-ment of Social Relations from 1967 to 1970. This was the time of student rebellion against the Vietnam War and elit-ism in the academy, and Roger could not avoid confronta-tions with students who transferred their unbound anger at society to anyone in a position of authority. I remember sitting in a large auditorium the afternoon Roger was speaking to hundreds of students waving flags and stamping their feet to complain about a policy that Roger had instituted. After a flow of abusive language, Roger replied, “I think that I make a very unlikely Fascist pig.” A long laugh broke the mob’s hostility and the afternoon was won. I dropped in on Roger later that day to congratulate him for creative handling of a messy situation.心理学空间2^ i-J'[ f f

心理学空间 L)qg5l s

Roger was remarkably active during the last two decades of his academic life. He wrote a draft of a book, never published, called, “A New Paradigm of Reference,” that re-vised earlier notions on meaning by accommodating to the work of his student Eleanor Rosch. He also composed thoughtful papers on the varied moods conveyed by music, the differences between novels and songs, and drafted a theory of politeness that profited from the work of Penelope Brown and Steven Levinson. Roger admitted to feeling de-lighted when he discovered that some verbs ascribe causal-ity to their targets—Bill admires Sam because Sam is admi-rable—whereas others ascribe causality to their subjects—Bill charms Sam because Bill is charming. However, he learned later that this provocative insight had been detected earlier by Alfonso Caramazza and Catherine Garvey.

Y7iW {"Z0s~C0心理学空间+C f7dEYQ'Y}9L

His health began to decline during the last decade of his life; he had a bypass operation and was diagnosed with prostate cancer. Despite the chronic discomfort of these illnesses, Roger retained his civility and gentleness, contin-ued to supervise students, and taught a popular course on the relation between psychology and fiction until retire-ment in 1994.

*L:Oa#J:o:A;[:j0

bVX8kmn)R Et0When Roger’s partner of forty years, Albert Gilman, died of cancer in 1989, he confessed to the unexpected diffi-culty of coping with the pain of that loss. During the next half dozen years, he tried to conquer each day’s dark mood of isolation with a series of relationships with younger men. He summarized those terrible years in a short book, pub-lished in 1996, called Against My Better Judgment. Roger’s friends were simultaneously puzzled and saddened by the honesty of this memoir and could not understand why he had chosen a confession that created impossible levels of dissonance in those who loved him most. Jean Berko Gleason, Roger’s first student, ended her comments in a 1998 me-morial service by saying that the Roger Brown she would remember was tall, handsome, brilliant, kind, incredibly generous, and a wickedly funny man.

_S;^l2g5tuy0心理学空间/} }:M4z0o)ydA+M

Eric Wanner, a former student and president of the Russell Sage Foundation, offered a speculation at the memorial service that may help to explain why Roger rubbed sadness into each page of the memoir. Wanner suggested that a suspicion of psychological theory and its unconstrained de-scriptors permeated Roger’s scholarship, and he wished to remind psychologists and his close friends of the easily sup-pressed truth that each person’s anima is so hidden and resistant to logical analysis, one must reject simple stereo-types as appropriate descriptions of the warren of feelings, thoughts, and symbols that are the skeleton of the human psyche. If one reads Against My Better Judgment with that assumption it becomes a revealing psychological document. I suspect this was one of Roger’s motivations for writing it.

#^`!}&Y]m0心理学空间(O0`k7u`'Lg

Roger Brown was a fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences and was elected to the National Academy of Sciences in 1972. He was awarded the G. Stanley Hall Prize in developmental psychology of the American Psycho-logical Association in 1973 and received honorary degrees from York University, England, in 1970, a D.Sc. from Bucknell University in 1980, and a D.Sc. from Northwestern Univer-sity in 1983. He was awarded the Distinguished Scientific Achievement Award of the American Psychological Associa-tion in 1971, and in 1985 was awarded the International Prize of the Foundation Fyssen in Paris.

rq)^'qY#E~]"Z0心理学空间 |T"uBFO m

【1 】THANK STEVEN PINKER for sharing with me his obituary to be pub-lished in Cognition in 1998.心理学空间 m]bva[!D ~

心理学空间6}T8n Dxed

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY

/|/md|HJf C$C0

*GGA*pyxsQ+d01954心理学空间{O)F%yZ SG `+X+[1f

心理学空间6u0^8\ vj/\!v

With E. Lenneberg. A study in language and cognition. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 49:454-62.心理学空间.l$cY.k2Q-^c*[/St9R

心理学空间1Cv.e^j6m9[/C

1958心理学空间@0^ksW3u$Yk!u:Y-~

l6ittx}0Words and Things: An Introduction to Language. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press.

3J-j.c}!R$gj_0

RxM3lft[0How shall a thing be called? Psychol. Rev. 65:14-21.

0{j@ S9Op d$A z*R0心理学空间tTO$f ?

1960

l}5a.a6x3}!~q!w0

$m,~ HvV$mg(X0With J. Berko. Psycholinguistic research methods. In Handbook of Research Methods in Child Psychology, ed. P. H. Mussen. New York: Wiley.心理学空间8w)m Y5a0C8Y)j

0_*`)O(M DPf8^01964

Wsnt{qs.H3Z*M0心理学空间 e-L)a*p_9iQ&I

With U. Bellugi. The acquisition of language. Monographs of the Soci-ety for Research in Child Development, No. 92.心理学空间7l\D,|KE y

心理学空间U3T] vY^

1966

3wK1NRX?(|0心理学空间-t e{6Y A

With D. McNeill. The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 5:325-37.心理学空间Npl2F'}{

v2F"l5GRHd$i0With A. Gilman. Personality and style in Concord. In Transcendental-ism and Its Legacy, eds. M. Simon and T. H. Parsons, pp. 87-122. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.心理学空间^T b,j%gNP

心理学空间"eRk1EGTLNF

1973心理学空间'L&b~ }-nY#S

G yl_dV1J|\9J0A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard Uni-versity Press. Development of the first language in the human species. Am. Psychol. 28:395-403.

5sh#g}2o@[Z|PV0心理学空间(a8~*O+g_{j L(@

1974

,sq `n9Nl!a0心理学空间qq V#eU#i7W5x

With R. Herrnstein. Psychology. Boston: Little Brown.心理学空间[ o$nN7c~

心理学空间+wn1j6x*q[$F

1976

2q&p n BA&V/p0

mv6EI[7@9a0Reference: In memorial tribute to Eric Lenneberg. Cognition 4:125-53. 1977 With J. Kulik. Flashbulb memories. Cognition 5:73-99.心理学空间;mUPp!i6l,g

心理学空间4Au*`&Ke~

1978心理学空间sll,A d4A8qp

u|5OOC~"[ b0A new paradigm of reference. In Psychology and Biology of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg, eds. G. A. Miller and E. Lenneberg. New York: Academic Press.

+]i \j.P!d1X0

+?gZ(Ir B%^ F)T3?01981

-a8k8P_*z5y~&|0

_.V:a;r4z%BtO0Cognitive categories. In Psychology’s Second Century: Enduring Issues, eds. R. A. Kasschau and C. N. Cofer, pp. 199-212. New York: Praeger. Symbolic and syntactic capacities. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 292:197-204.心理学空间Z"lM*_~]#dq.s

心理学空间7y+p PZ D,GL`

1983

/|5z qm!d}(J.g? J0心理学空间 e(O%n:w;DNZ

With D. Fish. The psychological causality implicit in language. Cog-nition. 14:237-73.

9^ ~G!i3m\V_]0心理学空间M*T*HHT!z5]6~&g-Y

1986

p1MM f!I;hz0_m%`0心理学空间D2g [gMP

Social Psychology: The Second Edition. New York: Free Press.

!h[f pJ0

eo4n S;fM5ZWp01989心理学空间.D D/n l!B

/O*gbCA;ro0A very private person. In A History of Psychology in Autobiography, vol. VIII, ed. G. Lindzey, pp. 47-59. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford Univer-sity Press.心理学空间,n%UZ?7BPK;Z:v9H[

YKe`7@.Y01996

~XsD+K)r&i9x u(z0心理学空间%PflP ?,[Ahw$v

Against My Better Judgment: An Intimate Memoir of an Eminent Gay Psychologist. New York and London: Harrington Park/Haworth Press.

F#a{"B TR.G*V0www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
TAG: 罗杰布朗
«罗杰·布朗 Roger William Brown 34 布朗 | Roger Brown
《34 布朗 | Roger Brown》
没有了»