The Birth of High Gods
作者: Shariff / 9650次阅读 时间: 2015年6月25日
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网心理学空间0_#AY'NhJ2J?

心理学空间u-W4QO7zBE3|

References

}G mo%N#^? j0

*|6ck/b+E l7wj^w0Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. Atran, S. (2002). In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. New York: Oxford University Press.心理学空间]L#aK3F5mt

XE g8tG[.e6M fx0Atran, S., & Norenzayan, A. (2004). Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 713–770.心理学空间!| k'b(m\ bY

7DX8`NeV+Z z}$])d0Atran, S., & Sperber, D. (1991). Learning without teaching. In L. Tolchinsky-Landsmann (Ed.), Culture, schooling and psychological development (pp. 39–55). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

'UfB%|D'YG0

\wSmjHc ln0Barrett, J. L., & Nyhof, M. A. (2001). Spreading non-natural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 69–100.心理学空间 e\8pK}L-_[

心理学空间 Xr j S@;} ~

Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2, 412–414.

5Y]%UR |#I0

5q$T1rH7G\ m0Bering, J. M. (2003, August). On reading symbolic random events: Children’s causal reasoning about unexpected occurrences. Paper presented at the Psychological and Cognitive Foundations of Religiosity Conference, Atlanta, GA.心理学空间,E2y fsUwN4^

心理学空间(P\4a7E j h}6D

Bering, J. M. (2006). The folk psychology of souls. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 453–462.

gF4edJ"pib0心理学空间qz"T,g%MLv\

Bloom, P. (2005). Is God an accident? Atlantic Monthly, 296(5), 105–112. Boesch, C., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591–614.

4I%SU(j&wO'a0

V{]"@ O%e#U2PVc0Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(2), 125–143.

jDr+o9R0

cn%w+i!PyY'dJk#KY0Boyer, P. (1996). What makes anthropomorphism natural: Intuitive ontology and cultural representations. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.), 2, 83–97. Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.心理学空间W.u`1J2?$hf!n!_T'j

心理学空间u7x Al!]%F/yj

Boyer, P., & Ramble, C. (2001). Cognitive templates for religious concepts: Cross-cultural evidence for recall of counter-intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25, 535–564.

!B3{o)i}&U+V0

5PkeM*d!Y0Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. New York: Oxford University Press.心理学空间 Kj*T;sw7Q dY9O

心理学空间R#ygE-m8Tc

Cauvin, J. (1999). The birth of the gods and the origins of agriculture (T. Watkins, Trans.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.心理学空间s {+i!u4Jl

心理学空间!Dg'~(V2p%lp

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). The social brain: Mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–181.心理学空间5_E3}`X1|Qq%J

J j"s&PfE NF+n2C0Durkheim, E. (1912/1995). The elementary forms of religious life. New York: Free Press.心理学空间 G+bK4g0Rg

心理学空间!^C7ftpa;jc

Edgell, P., Gerteis, J., & Hartmann, D. (2006). Atheists as “other”: Moral boundaries and cultural membership in American society. American Sociological Review, 71(2), 211–239.心理学空间Hi an8YdQ!Fe Y

g*I(_.qH Ec'_$y0Fodor, J.A. (1987). Modules, frames, fridgeons, sleeping dogs and the music of the spheres. In J. Garfield (Ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding (pp. 25-36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

cN] A4H Dp0

s hH,oe&~zd)Q0Gervais, W., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). Beware the infidel: The implicit mistrust of atheists. Unpublished manuscript.心理学空间Wz*sO3Y)\

v}p#|8x0Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (in press). The moral mind: How 5 sets of innate moral intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules.

I6?H\_*g0

_uI/Z7g0In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind (Vol. 3).

WR5vy o1@\Uge0心理学空间_tWS6e'z L

Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 245–256.

w_ ?)v9bT-{,Ih\Y0

wj(vtuOf9x0Harris, E., & McNamara, P. (2008). Is religiousness a biocultural adaptation? In J. Bulbulia, R. Sosis, E. Harris, R. Genet, C. Genet, & K. Wyman, (Eds.), The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques (pp. 79–85). Santa Margarita, CA: Collins Foundation Press.心理学空间0Gi&O~,~^qD

心理学空间w7kbm"u

Henrich, J. (2006). Cooperation, punishment, and the evolution of human institutions. Science, 312, 60–61.心理学空间{,@@ hks

心理学空间/Vd9y'n Q L

Henrich, J. (2007). The evolutionary links between cooperation, religion, and ritual. Unpublished manuscript.心理学空间2S8Tm}B

hI[#^v Y0Henrich, J. (in press). A cultural species. In M. Brown (Ed.), Explaining culture scientifically. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

+jUx L_2W1s0

a}jO }7X)^VZ0Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–242.心理学空间 rK0@GG

S _J ZM0Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 208, 79–89.心理学空间T&Y5?1s9g,Cg

0f+C x(LLY0Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred status as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 1–32.

D1{w%erD8QdNN0心理学空间 FojH K/b_:y"~

Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2006). Dual inheritance theory: The evolution of human cultural capacities and cultural evolution. In R. Dunbar & L. Barrett (Eds.), Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 555–570). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.心理学空间+bN/~K%["g

8mb-w ?)mrp0Henrich, N. S., & Henrich, J. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.心理学空间[ f/`g-J;~+{` ?

心理学空间ai9Y)Xs"R6L9vV

Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346–380.心理学空间C.z.l'T(?5Af

心理学空间%L^G.vz I\

Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249–251.  

X!tjU9x0心理学空间|j6KEP@

Irons, W. (1991). How did morality evolve? Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science, 26, 49–89.心理学空间H2E#N:F4bL6}

cEd YdmYA0Johnson, A. (2003). Families of the forest: Matsigenka Indians of the Peruvian Amazon. Berkeley: University of California Press.

"V}pm1XQmC0心理学空间Y3h[o_

Johnson, D.P. (2008). The supernatural punishment hypothesis. Oral presentation at The Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Meeting. Albuquerque, New Mexico. (February, 2008).

3F0j1C'PY0c[&?0

};[0[5F6g%[0Johnson, D. D. P., & Bering, J. M. (2006). Hand of God, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 219–233.

`!|qlY @ D8Ny l0心理学空间b{-Q1z^1h%E\y

Johnson, D. D. P., & Krueger, O. (2004). Supernatural punishment and the evolution of cooperation. Political Theology, 5(2), 159–176.

%R4Le_R-{5{.}4C+PC0心理学空间/b e4A(UN_r}NMa

Kelly, R. L. (1995). The foraging spectrum: Diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Press.

D1|aPpL[+~0心理学空间']9C+c^3^w

Kirkpatrick, L. (1999). Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion. Journal of Personality, 67, 921–952.心理学空间q,Mrs.fVw

+k Je@C0Landau, M. J., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (2005). The motivational underpinnings of religion. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 743–744.心理学空间rsS*oA\E+OBj

;[;` u\9_SC0Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2001). Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocity. Proceedings: Biological Sciences, 268(1468), 745–753.

sVw;](`2s]0

oS UN_;R/q5E&b0McGrew, W. C. (1974). Tool use by wild chimpanzees feeding on driver ants. Journal of Human Evolution, 3, 501–508.

G~$I/~u2d+jO c0心理学空间'{$j:PHOm

McNeill, W. H. (1991). The rise of the West: A history of the human community. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.心理学空间0Kg{Lg

P!@x?!? j5pF+V0Neudecker, S. (1989). Eye comouflage and false eyespots: Chaetodontid responses to predators. Environmental Biology of Fishes, 25, 143–157.心理学空间sX:d }$TA$h

bd,Xd ~5o!aVBO!{ a0Norenzayan, A., Atran, S., Faulkner, J., & Schaller, M. (2006). Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science, 30, 531–553.

'qng6p%?.N}#jq$}q0

Y3|3c_n0Norenzayan, A., Schaller, M., & Heine, S. (2006). Evolution and culture. In M. Schaller, J. Simpson, & D. Kenrick (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 343–366). New York: Psychology Press.

K3u a.c[:A0

*C@ G)a9Hr,HO0Norenzayan, A., & Shariff, A. F. (2008). The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Unpublished manuscript, University of British Columbia.心理学空间#A+L&pwuA$w @.S

H0B.j)\X y(V0Nowak, M. A. (2006). Evolutionary dynamics: Exploring the equations of life. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.心理学空间{LaV4C9K

心理学空间 fs8kcaftry-}

Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291–1298.心理学空间@ rc"H)I/tf

-Km%_ n wc q&|O.O |/r0Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2003). A tale of two defectors: The importance of standing for the evolution of indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 224, 115–126.

*slHM}0心理学空间FcZ@+f F

Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432, 499–502.心理学空间rH9~c5fw7Fg!X

dVeT)SZ6i A0Panksepp, J., Panksepp, J.B. (2000). The seven sins of evolutionary psychology. Evolution and Cognition, 6, 108–131.

QM9^ ~1]9D0

-kpW.}PM0Price, D. T., & Brown, J. A. (Eds.). (1988). Prehistoric hunter gathers: The emergence of cultural complexity. New York: Academic Press.

7D0H;dIJ1\0

h$g0zx2H ] mQB0Rendell, L. E., & Whiten, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–382.

9yPGl K1Y%R#AGW0

$u:U|&DWX m0Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of ultrasociality. In I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt & F. K. Salter (Eds.), Indoctrinability, ideology and warfare (pp. 71–96). New York: Berghahn Books.心理学空间#V@{NK

OU2d9q E7sh'pr~0Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.心理学空间0w}!A$ZM^Y'Bc4j

D5t e6G*l8|0Rigaud, M. (1985). Secrets of voodoo (R. B. Cross, Trans.). San Francisco: City Light Books.  

+D;@eY-b"\ O*G6?Yz0心理学空间\9Y? e p

Roes, F., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 126–135.

d;qZkMs5@#sb3_0心理学空间%@8RYix#E9}Ne,[

Rogers, A. A. (1988). Does biology constrain culture? American Anthropologist, 90, 819–831.心理学空间S:r"a3VvDoh%M(ty+i;N

心理学空间0q5FS NP3wp-MB9Y(]

Rozin, P. (1987). Psychobiological perspectives on food preferences and avoidances. In M. Harris & E. B. Ross (Eds.), Food and evolution: Toward a theory of human food habits (pp. 181–205). Philadelphia: Temple University Press.

.b` k#Lh5zl-B M]0

;|rd m$kiR0Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–809.心理学空间s(eiGl8` h.Zom

!Um1f+] bt0Sherman, P. W., & Billing, J. (1999). Darwinian gastronomy: Why we use spices. BioScience, 49, 453–463.

6p"x _#n R$yK#]+I0

z'ThOet0Smith, R. J. (1996). Biology and body size in human evolution: Statistical inference misapplied. Current Anthropology, 37(3), 451–481.

5R `eSe _0_P0D;]\0

Y#p&DI/|9dT0Snarey, J. (1996). The natural environment’s impact upon religious ethics: A cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 80, 85–96.心理学空间.q1}:GKSui

心理学空间/r;l!Lo8L

Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264–274.心理学空间,Z If:vAZ4Op lBU

心理学空间uy r,Pw*h[

Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. R. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37(2), 211–239.

;|4Q-m#u2k S A%[ A0

5Y*]u ye-Gp0Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44, 713–722.心理学空间e,b YP a w$D(B$T(^n

心理学空间3?1H${ k)a

Stevens, M. (2005). The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573–588.

Y uk$\&K0心理学空间n${b1r#t{$k9q

Tan, J. H. W., & Vogel, C. (2005, September). Religion and trust: An experimental study. Paper presented to the annual congress of the International Association for Research in Economic Psychology, Prague.

sq!fI{s0心理学空间6e'`|)dV)BN

Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

g0e}lw/H)bf+{0心理学空间.`G D'i!t iO

Tomasello, M., Kruger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–511.

W'v5a1hK~T'b ^0

+Uv"{5ar0IBt0Tuzin, D. (1976). The Ilahita Arapesh. Berkeley: University of California Press. Tuzin, D. (2001). Social complexity in the making: A case study among the Arapesh of New Guinea. London: Routledge.

9]\+eC0t0

~AK\DExiZ4[9r0Voltaire, F. M. A. D., & Redman, B. R. (1977). The portable Voltaire. New York: Penguin Books.心理学空间J Kq} R

心理学空间%U/|aGrK}

Williams, G. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

-y2R1u!Qn4w*[0

*vd H{7Hk XI0Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.心理学空间`,Q9xX5a*W,C%B!n5`@

心理学空间Ryfy$qB

Wrangham, R., & Conklin-Brittain, N. (2003). Cooking as a biological trait. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular and Integrative Physiology, 136, 35–46.

Y)U@HR0www.psychspace.com心理学空间网
«探究宗教与利社会行为之间关系 Ara Norenzayan 阿兰•洛伦萨杨
《Ara Norenzayan 阿兰•洛伦萨杨》
解构宗教与社会进化关系:大社会需要大神明»