THE BRITISH OBJECT RELATIONS SCHOOL: W. R. D. FAIRBAIRN
作者: Mitchell / 22499次阅读 时间: 2012年11月25日
来源: Freud and Beyond 标签: FAIRBAIRN Fairbairn 精神分析 客体关系
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网心理学空间*QVH(oz o&mU!f

心理学空间s'E+E_@.K0uq

心理学空间` ar5Vw&m!MR6i?

THE BRITISH OBJECT RELATIONS SCHOOL: W. R. D. FAIRBAIRN 心理学空间.f'h?y%D!C&M

心理学空间 npl#J2dy

O6twY/z0

$U%}.BjHhRso0We only become what we are by the radical and deep-seated refusal of that which others have made of us.心理学空间o c+|p(i^ |'nR

axk+zE0-- Jean-Paul Sartre心理学空间aM S&n5rn7R

*er/iadQ8l1[,~Y0For Freud, in short, man was the ambivalent animal; for Winnicott, he would be the dependent animal. . . . Prior to sexuality as the unacceptable, there was helplessness. Dependence was the first thing, before good and evil.

1O,K+MoZ]XA3?2iQ!WN0心理学空间 o;rq(Fj6\w/U0}

-- Adam Phillips

KO4L/Q7m5Hk1vhcw'O0心理学空间.Cn/g3[H]rV d_ g

Human beings, in Freud's account, are born at odds with their environment. They are wired the way Freud and his contemporaries understood animals to be, oriented toward pursuing simple pleasures with ruthless abandon. But in Freud's Hobbesian view of human society, the individual's pursuit of egoistic satisfactions endangers other individuals, necessitating a control by the group on the hedonic aims of each individual. The project of childhood is socialization, the transforma-

1Vq2`K](e0tion of the infant, with his or her bestial impulses, into the adult, with his or her complex psychic apparatus and its intricate and elaborate system of checks and barriers channeling those impulses and aims into socially acceptable forms of civilized living.心理学空间'z$y3TTf qP)q

Q8u'v ]0I|A)L-r:t0心理学空间+qB a?_ |5|

All the important contemporary psychoanalytic schools view the human infant as less alien, more suited and adapted to the world into which he or she is born, than Freud did. As we have seen in chapter 2, Heinz Hartmann's concept of adaptation provided the central conceptual vehicle for the transition from Freud to contemporary Freudian ego psychology. It was Melanie Klein who provided the crucial bridge between Freud and modern British object relations theories.心理学空间5N m-_&Qp6DV

心理学空间ScnP;L q

In redefining the nature of "drive" to include built-in human objects, Klein fundamentally altered the basic premises and metaphors underlying psychoanalytic theorizing. Freud envisioned a developmental passage from animal to person. Klein portrayed a distinctly human infant from the start, an infant who does not learn about the breast through "accidental" association, but who instinctively knows about the breast because she is born with that knowledge. Just as the infant's mouth is anatomically shaped to fit the mother's nipple, the infant's instinctual impulses are shaped to fit the distinctively human world into which she is born.

2N8S^:Iu@:H0

Iq"Vv Z4Fim0Yet Klein's baby is not a very happy baby. This baby is born with the capacity to organize discomfort and pain into an image of a persecutory, "bad" Other, and to organize comfort and pleasure and pain into an image of a rescuing, "good" Other. Because early experiences collect around prewiredobjects, constitutional patterns of danger and refuge, that infancy is inevitably fragmented and terrifying. For Klein, the project of childhood is not socialization, but amelioration of the frightening, nightmarish conditions of the infant's experience of being in the world which derive from the intensity of the infant's needs and the overwhelming force of constitutional aggression. We are born with psychotic anxieties and, under favorable conditions, sanity becomes a developmental achievement. Although pre-wired to draw her into the human environment, the infant's instincts, in Klein's account, generate inevitable and considerable psychic pain, which, under favorable circumstances, can be contained, organized and assuaged by good parenting.

^q2e5WMr#@%aP0心理学空间 t`1Z@YO%W

In the early 1940s, after decades of acrimonious debate, the British Psychoanalytic Society split into three groups: those who fully accepted Melanie Klein's innovations in theory and technique; those who remained loyal to more traditional Freudian concepts and practice (headed by Anna Freud and extended into Freudian ego psychology); and an "independent" or middle group, who developed non-Kleinian versions of what came to be known as object relations theories. The major figures in this middle group, W. R. D. Fairbairn, D. W. Winnicott, Michael Balint, John Bowlby, and Harry Guntrip, all built on Klein's vision of an infant wired for human interaction. Yet they also all broke with Klein's premise of constitutional aggression deriving from the death instinct, proposing instead an infant wired for harmonious interaction and nontraumatic development but thwarted by inadequate parenting.心理学空间T$xA$CY8p N%DO.p

.?&e`-R+_pv0In recalling his early days in the British Psychoanalytic Society, when the Kleinian belief in the aggressive, destructive baby predominated, John Bowlby cited as a milestone in the emergence of his own independent line of thought the moment when he rose defiantly to assert, in the middle of

(g/LmT F3r0one such discussion, "But there is such a thing as a bad mother." That succinct statement might serve as banner heralding in the development of postKleinian British object relations theories.1心理学空间$joD*{5z9Dr

W. R. D. FAIRBAIRN心理学空间9Tmh*YPL*Cx:\}

心理学空间 rpJs`+x9D6HB#jZ

Freud's clinical observations charted the vicissitudes of human misery, the ways people systematically make themselves unhappy over and over again: thesymptom neurosis, in which compulsive, bizarre behavior intrudes into experience; the character neurosis, in which maladaptive, self-defeating patterns of behavior compromise interactions with others; thefate neurosis, in which the same self-destructive destiny is orchestrated repeatedly;depression, in which emotional pain is perpetually regenerated. Yet Freud's broad motivational theory, anchored in his concepts of instinctual drive and the pleasure principle, is a hedonic theory: People seek pleasure and avoid pain. The motivational framework of drive theory is very difficult to reconcile with Freud's clinical observations of therepetition compulsion, the systematic regeneration of distress: painful symptoms, painful patterns of behavior, painful fates, painful affective states. If people are meant to seek pleasure and avoid pain, why are most of us so extraordinarily competent at keeping ourselves unhappy?心理学空间r:mmQs qz

心理学空间1p;G)sA \ \

According to the pleasure principle, libido is malleable, employing a variety of interchangeable objects in its pursuit of pleasure; it ought to be able to discard painful desires and frustrating objects. Yet, Freud noted in 1905, the libido also has a property he termed adhesiveness, which seems to operate at odds with the pleasure principle. Libido gets painfully stuck to old, inaccessible objects, frustrated longings, thwarted desires. The Oedipus complex, the heart of Freud's clinical theory, is the prime example of this. Freud returned to this knotty problem again and again, in his efforts to explain nightmares (since he understood dreams as wish fulfillments), sexual masochism (since he understood sexuality as the pursuit of pleasure, not pain), traumatic neurosis (in which terrifying experiences are impossible to leave behind).2www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

TAG: FAIRBAIRN Fairbairn 精神分析 客体关系
«费尔贝恩的学说 by 李孟潮 费尔贝恩 Fairbairn
《费尔贝恩 Fairbairn》
Psychoanalytic Studies of the Personality人格的精神分析研究»