THE ORIGINS OF ATTACHMENT THEORY: JOHN BOWLBY AND MARY AINSWORTH
作者: INGE BRETHERTON / 36158次阅读 时间: 2011年4月24日
来源: Developmental Psychology (1992), 28, 759-775.
www.psychspace.com心理学空间网心理学空间)Y1K"aP2j"W

NEW DIRECTIONS
ZhU B |,z0心理学空间n'[R-Ba"tby
Currently, attachment theory and research are moving forward along several major fronts, 心理学空间0U.H:rj/WG"t0e
inspired by the second and third volumes of Bowlby’s attachment trilogy, by methodological 心理学空间|PU1VJ i7T
advances, and by the infusion into attachment theory of complementary theoretical perspectives.
utI!nB6z'\0心理学空间kW ad%|&|
Attachment and Representation
6R?aJ"hF0心理学空间O3h/tQ9f6aJ:_m
As a result of Mary Main’s Berkeley study (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985) and, I think, the 心理学空间0t7Ic(k s&A.ht4}*i
心理学空间l^?x(d i

ak.w Ug1d1gHY1`0publication of the Society for Research in Child Development Monograph, Growing Points of 心理学空间/hrK]&TY
Attachment Theory and Research (Bretherton & Waters, 1985), we are now beginning to
nGc6S%HS'r0empirically explore the psychological, internal, or representational aspects of attachment, including
5{|(s#Y1M1r0the intergenerational transmission of attachment patterns that had been at the center of 心理学空间Va$mc8{
Bowlby’s interests since his beginnings in psychiatry but that are most clearly elaborated in
`1G1_!g|k7}J7k s0volumes 2 and 3 of the attachment trilogy (see Bretherton, 1987, 1990, 1991).
2R2v\;H9CIp}0
k3mK?.Xu]o9K GP0Interestingly, an additional source of inspiration for the study of internal working models
P]N_Z+zg0came from attempts to translate Ainsworth’s infant- mother attachment patterns into corresponding
:l[o(m5{zD0adult patterns. in the Adult Attachment Interview (George, Kaplan, & Main, 1984; Main &
3e+f(|_7t5A0Goldwyn, in press), parents were asked open-ended questions about their attachment relations in
(\*TXb2L0childhood and about the influence of these early relations on their own development. Three 心理学空间"HU5{m|!nB"M'V
distinct patterns of responding were identified: Autonomous-secure parents gave a clear and 心理学空间j)E(V+^0LX7R3i
coherent account of early attachments (whether these had been satisfying or not); preoccupied 心理学空间$T F@7N}.UHJ
parents spoke of many conflicted childhood memories about attachment but did not draw them
GT%G-c:Y.|$]7O+v0together into an organized, consistent picture; and, finally, dismissing parents were characterized 心理学空间jT2o G(Ce
by an inability to remember much about attachment relations in childhood. In some of the 心理学空间 kfL LU@/m
dismissing interviews, parents’ parents were idealized on a general level, hut influences of early 心理学空间7D;p P"sk4Y+}f tg)k B
attachment experiences on later development were denied. Specific memories, when they did
|#t9f)C%Z,@+Fn*i0occur, suggested episodes of rejection. 心理学空间P+Jb0\ R'q2Q4CTu{4]

_\b4}oF0Not only did the Adult Attachment Interview classifications correspond to Ainsworth’s 心理学空间~_a"j&seXV0RP
secure, ambivalent, and avoidant infant patterns at a conceptual level, but adult patterns were also
8y&L7|@cthx!c0empirically correlated with infant patterns (e.g., a dismissing parent tended to have an avoidant
6cC? u`~CP8^0infant; Main & Goldwyn, in press). These findings have since been validated for prenatally 心理学空间PwV o8AE&I*m
administered interviews by Fonagy, Steele, and Steele (1991) and by Ward et al. (1990). 心理学空间ngA!W%op S Z
Consonant findings were also obtained in a study of young adults in which Adult Attachment
4P?h} db0Interview classifications were correlated with peer reports (Kobak & Sceery, 1988).
#T1BO9D-PG0
]tL's,J3~q{0In addition, representational measures of attachment have been devised for use with
/@ uz$S-l5r0children. A pictorial separation anxiety test for adolescents, developed by Hansburg (1972), was 心理学空间.r|RJ)Xj5? c

,F:[O*P9~ g/W9hV0
_#i%bHHc8g0adapted for younger children by Klagsbrun and Bowlby (1976) and more recently revised and 心理学空间xx1o2|8^X3m8Y
validated against observed attachment patterns by Kaplan (1984) and Slough and Greenberg 心理学空间 `(Z;if&I
(1991) Likewise, attachment-based doll story completion tasks for preschoolers were validated
?4a+~m y0against behavioral measures by Bretherton, Ridgeway, and Cassidy (1990) and Cassidy (1988). In 心理学空间%H}l;p"z7j6_
these tests, emotionally open responding tended to be associated with secure attachment 心理学空间9g O#I6~U9V
classifications or related behaviors. 心理学空间o];eF:gv
心理学空间/a*f)E+`5i
Finally, several authors have created interviews that examine attachment from the parental 心理学空间E8C;|MZ*W1N d!J}
as opposed to the filial perspective (e.g., Bretherton, Biringen, Ridgeway, Maslin, & Sherman,
0a+?;J.Q*E&[{01989; George & Solomon, 1989). In addition, Waters and Q-sort that can be used to assess a 心理学空间:|Z#q&Z9t [%f2H
mother’s internal working models of her child’s attachment to her.
^&Rl;OM k#f0心理学空间(b.Dfcx"LS2Q
Attachment Across the Life Span
{ `&C gw1y:O8I0心理学空间R Z2O T~V{
A related topic, attachment relationships between adults, began in the early 1970s, with 心理学空间Rf(Y W2{,s;WS
studies of adult bereavement (Bowlby & Parkes, 1970; Parkes, 1972) and marital separation
)H#o3~u/Fg"l4G,^0(Weiss, 1973, 1977). More recently, interest in adult attachments has broadened to encompass
qT| yY%l,Fb0marital relationships (Weiss, 1982, 1991) and has taken a further upsurge with work by Shaver 心理学空间[6Ve.Ca)o8\1]
and Hazan (1988), who translated Ainsworth’s infant attachment patterns into adult patterns, 心理学空间x | u-Q^?W
pointing out that adults who describe themselves as secure, avoidant, or ambivalent with respect 心理学空间&Z"iP|2^
to romantic relationships report differing patterns of parent-child relationships in their families of
;Jf1QT@2[0origin. Finally, Cicirelli (1989, 1991) has applied attachment theory to the study of middle-aged
mL PokD&uo0siblings and their elderly parents. Much future work will be needed to delineate more fully the 心理学空间3g[ \"_,{
distinct qualities of child-adult, child-child, and adult-adult attachment relationships (see 心理学空间.l;nL lv[LYU
Ainsworth, 1989), as well as their interplay within the family system, a task begun by Byng-Hall
~ba&v0b0(1985) and Marvin and Stewart (1990),
6u6Fu'RM+zO0心理学空间&C7Vom-p
Attachment and Developmental Psychopathology 心理学空间[1n7]_ F
心理学空间^D!o_v3p^7o/]
Attachment theory and research are also making a notable impact on the emerging field of
K!oXX'N&F0developmental psychopathology (Sroufe, 1988), with longitudinal attachment-based studies of 心理学空间 hGs:]+ra
families with depression (Radke-Yarrow, Cummings, Kuczinsky, & Chapman, 1985), of families 心理学空间y4_!K p8CK
with maltreatment (e.g., Cicchetti & Barnett, 1991; Crittenden, 1983; Schneider-Rosen, Braunwald,
2Iw"KIMf3B6c4pf+c[VX0Carlson, & Cicchetti, 1985), and of clinical interventions in families with low social support
`_0w4^ t,i@x&K)Qg0
^4Kq_RR!?0心理学空间E/`5`sa:Blp
(Lieberman & Pawl, 1988; Spieker & Booth, 1988) and with behavior-problem children 心理学空间8`tS3Hb:O$]I Z
(Greenberg & Speltz, 1988). Much of this work is represented in a volume on clinical
,E8K zq%uM0implications of attachment (Belsky & Nezworski, 1988). These topics hark back to Bowlby’s
D c-m [X.wOk pDz0seminal ideas from the 1930s, but they have been greatly enriched by Mary Ainsworth’s notions
+UB%s/a%p+R7U0on the origins of individual differences of attachment patterns. 心理学空间)|]6]3y5lw@ D{']3P

2xQ5t}8M1P0The Ecology of Attachment
s2Me V${5n.L0
Ivp"u1a!Y,B0Although we have made progress in examining mother-child attachment, much work needs 心理学空间V)}(DMDG v/JH
to he done with respect to studying attachment in the microsystem of family relationships
E"R%@u d7z,is,s0(Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Despite studies by Belsky, Gilstrap, and Rovine (1984), Lamb (1978), 心理学空间X0`/|e vt
and Parke and Tinsley (1987) that show fathers to be competent, if sometimes less than fully
b#pc6q,L&Xe0participant attachment figures, we still have much to learn regarding father attachment. Another 心理学空间 `3kdN(YY#p2D
important topic, sibling attachment, has been tackled by a few researchers (e.g., Stewart & 心理学空间)p0Z,c0y:S \Kw
Marvin, 1984; Teti & Ablard, 1989), but triadic studies of attachment relationships (modeled on
C YKwF8kLI/CC0Dunn, 1988) are sorely lacking. Especially crucial are attachment-theoretic studies of loyalty
a Seyy)Rr m0conflicts, alliances by a dyad vis-a-vis a third family member, and enmeshment of a child in the 心理学空间\7a]/e{(un1B
spousal dyad, as exemplified in a report by Fish, Belsky, and Youngblade (1991) in which
#l'B h:v R|e b#L9P5r H6Ya0insecure attachment in infancy was associated with inappropriate involvement in spousal decision-心理学空间5R)PQ!p V#z+q4M,qT
making at 4 years of age. Finally, the interrelations of child temperament and developing
#QDy#G1]0attachment relationships with other family members remain conceptually unclear despite intensive 心理学空间1B0G6w!`:k9[v7P
research efforts (Belsky & Rovine, 1987; Sroufe, 1985).
w)k[7Lmy`w4F0
P&WL#A*K Ty0The documentation of family and social network factors as they affect attachment relations
:g [?6UIo0(e.g., Belsky & Isabella, 1988; Belsky, Rovine, & Taylor, 1984) has been more successful. In the 心理学空间 u!Aa6lHH
Pennsylvania project, attachment quality at the end of the first year was predictable from relative
6sXYW5MVy~`0changes in levels of marital satisfaction after the child’s birth, as well as from parental satisfaction
Fxkx h|z`ZJ0with social support, hut not its frequency.
YE7b/Ya5R+i0
.hQm5zemTrm0An ecological perspective also calls for an examination of issues related to dual-worker
3@'t iy\0families, especially in view of the continued sex/gender differentiation of parenting. Some feminist 心理学空间 E T y }~mT2m
theorists have interpreted attachment theory as supporting the traditional view of women as 心理学空间0~hL*XOK
primary caregivers (Chodorow, 1978; Johnson, 1988). This is not strictly justified, because 心理学空间%q-t.@ b3SOXq

%Wa^#RO$cGB0
!f.a/X[1H/o0attachment theory does not specify that caregiving must be done by mothers or be restricted to
2ia |7\}&}*b.i q0females (Marris, 1982), Most central to healthy development, according to attachment theory, is 心理学空间"Ls}3Jo x
infants’ need for a committed caregiving relationship with one or a few adult figures. Although 心理学空间 F#G"e%n j!m{EII
the majority of attachment studies have focused on mothers because mothers tend to fill this role 心理学空间U,Z(e e,}#ez
most often, we do have evidence that infants can he attached to a hierarchy of figures, including
xH_ D$M0fathers, grandparents, and siblings (Schaffer & Emerson, 1964), as well as to day-care providers
:t+N`e)oy N9z"e0(Howes, Rodning, Galuzzo, & Myers, 1988). However, our knowledge about the range of 心理学空间|4YAs%LL&_
societal options for successfully sharing the task of bringing up children is still woefully 心理学空间 N)eCj0?,W-l2V
inadequate. The recent spate of studies documenting an increased risk of insecure attachment if
%qj w9r[ C.]9d0day care begins in the first year and is extensive in duration (Belsky & Rovine, 1988; Belsky & 心理学空间&\dzIGQ.bx
Braungart, 1991) is worrisome and needs resolution. Cross-cultural studies of attachment and 心理学空间z J @},s)PiQ
nonparental care in countries such as Sweden and Israel may ultimately provide more reliable 心理学空间1fx$q[V8\
answers. 心理学空间oFe n-c0} K2Jc'H:u
心理学空间 U&amiX&Y{8S'A#N2E
Cross-Cultural Studies 心理学空间g;r1s#C1|'F4Gb.jo

U b'bP2O&V dq6v0Moving from family and other social networks to the larger societal matrix, we find that 心理学空间&Rmc!ARS6K?
studies of Strange Situation classifications in other cultures have sparked a lively debate on their 心理学空间p8J#Pz\l9o
universal versus culture-specific meaning. in a north German study, avoidant classifications were 心理学空间n#Is9}!L s7iAY$X
overrepresented (Grossmann, Grossmann, Spangler, Suess, & Unzner, 1985), whereas ambivalent
1z:oQ"f]&H%l8cxCZ0classifications were more frequent than expected in Israeli kibbutzim (Sagi et al., 1985) and 心理学空间'i*O9q m SI3j4N0fV
in Japan (Miyake, Chen, & Campos, 1985).
!wru3],Ai i0
Rv0w"{5H)r3T$f{&m:`0Initially, these findings were interpreted in purely cultural terms. Thus, Grossmann et al. 心理学空间+x#?DjF|!ZVw
(1985) proposed that the high incidence of avoidant infants in Germany should be attributed not 心理学空间)Y u9o{A#|@.x+h't7D
to parental rejection, hut rather to a greater parental push toward infants’ independence.
7}u8f`}V lz Vl0Similarly, the high frequency of ambivalent classifications observed in Israeli kibbutzim and Japan 心理学空间 _l B$P5nw$i._
was attributed to underexposure to strangers (Miyake et al., 1985; Sagi et al., 1985). Though
"Om0x8NGC+X{0persuasive on the surface, these explanations were not based on systematic assessments of
+fZo_Q2h G#|7EW H0parental beliefs and culturally guided practices. 心理学空间x1n&z-XvTL
心理学空间5gW0`G+n A2yZ{9\6o
More recently, van Ijzendoorn and Kroonenberg (1988) examined the frequency distributions
h$vc#v/J/| WZ0of Strange Situation classifications from over a thousand U.S. and cross-national studies, 心理学空间|!aJHf(_p0J

,{Y.k%z!pD2]K}&g0
|z!ChxA l-f%nL0pointing out that valid conclusions about cross-national differences should not be drawn from
"tF%?*M8F `d l0single samples. In addition, intercorrelational patterns of home and Strange Situation behavior in 心理学空间U9T sp O7K
north Germany (Grossmann et al., 1985) closely resembled those in the Ainsworth’s Baltimore
fGTs D0study, at least in part undermining a purely cultural interpretation. Likewise, Sagi, Aviezer,
N4kyqY CtJ0Mayseless, Donnell, and Joels (1991) attribute the abundance of ambivalent classifications to 心理学空间4^S)kp+M7~
specific nighttime caregiving arrangements in the kibbutzim they studied, rather than fewer 心理学空间I\0Q-Jqs
experiences with strangers. Taken in combination, these findings suggest that Strange Situation 心理学空间-]:i(V(h(~r
classifications, and hence the concept of parental sensitivity, may have more cross-cultural
L7|\1dK:Lz0validity in industrialized nations than was initially believed, hut the issue is by no means resolved.
1hX$vsD [3x0心理学空间d0Z!o~L!z:C
Systematic work on the more fascinating topic of how different cultures-especially non-
@.D7R%rB1H0Western cultures-fit attachment behaviors and relationships into their overall social organization 心理学空间Bk{} }Nh
has barely begun. There are, however, some tantalizing hints in the ethnographic literature (see
p2v Nbw k%VFG?D1a0Bretherton, 1985, for a review). For example, the Micronesian society of Tikopia (Firth, 1936)
(M`}$k6Sc Dj q0deliberately fosters attachment between an infant and its maternal uncle by prescribing face-toface 心理学空间)X@s8io:_"j9R8mKP
talk with the infant on a regular basis. This maternal uncle is destined to play an important 心理学空间%Y_6h6U3S/DbZv
quasi-parental role in the life of the child. Along somewhat different lines, Balinese mothers
Q5n%lS k8[*g3`0control their infants’ exploratory behavior by using fake fear expressions to bring the infants back
6sD|J_0into close proximity to them (Bateson & Mead, 1942). In both cultures, a biological system is
8?[ {%F6Y0molded to a particular society’s purposes (by fostering specific relationships or controlling 心理学空间a6mv~+C
exploration). 心理学空间H.h6R}]2]z5K
心理学空间(H/H,`&X\y%xV$y.q
A recent study of parent-infant attachment among the Efe begins to provide systematic
5`!bz;[&T8L9zWl*v/]9n)i0information in this area. The Efe, a semi-nomadic people, live in the African rain forest, subsisting 心理学空间DB+?7g e3X!lh
on foraging, horticulture, and hunting (Tronick, Winn, & Morelli, 1985). Young Efe infants 心理学空间AQ9v9?}
receive more care (including nursing) from other adult women than from their own mother, 心理学空间0Hbq ])C
except at night. Despite this multiple mothering system, by 6 months, infants begin to insist on a
g-CRLQ0more focalized relationship with their own mothers, although other female caregivers continue to 心理学空间"M y+f'`8v;}(C
play a significant role. Tronick et al. attributed Efe practices to their living arrangements, with
w#iZ6N"o/Rb0closely spaced dwellings that offer little privacy and that make cooperation and sharing highly 心理学空间0X8{v O%V-hXP1K
valued behaviors. In sum, attachment behavior is heavily overlain with cultural prescriptions, even 心理学空间$Fc8\o'Mjl

8b `]8e7q0
^,?+OETME0in a society that much more closely resembles the conditions of human evolution than our own. 心理学空间.Tj\ G:k?J/F
To better explore such cultural variations in attachment organization attachment researchers need
@vb*YM0to develop ecologically valid, theory-driven measures, tailored to specific cultures and based on a
*JF8o~,i Z*[0deeper knowledge of parents’ and children’s culture-specific folk theories about family relationships 心理学空间 _HT7PH'b
and attachment.
bC2@+Ai%R0心理学空间#M*US.f3XH^ h+I'Ej
Attachment and Public Policy 心理学空间D'ci+G C[u

h8iiG^&I-H'vP+X0Cultural differences in the regulation of attachment behaviors raise important questions 心理学空间 \G2X0_/d7D|X5{
about the value diverse societies place on attachment relations. In a thought-provoking chapter, 心理学空间?3o%e:no W1]m-v^
Marris (1991) points to the fundamental tension between the desire to create a secure and 心理学空间 [0B ?$@&w(j vJ q
predictable social order and the desire to maximize one’s own opportunities at the expense of
E'uOXk(E.V#P'Y0others. A good society, according to Marris, would he one which, as far as is humanly possible,
'nST9U.~3@0minimizes disruptive events, protects each child’s experience of attachment from harm, and 心理学空间!Q4anz3v&kGUeT
supports family coping. Yet, in order to control uncertainty, individuals and families are tempted
J)d$axMr0E2@0to achieve certainty at the expense of others (i.e., by imposing a greater burden of uncertainty on 心理学空间5wQ\9p~ls
them or by providing fewer material and social resources). When powerful groups in society
0cE~H(B/KLy0promote their own control over life circumstances by subordinating and marginalizing others,
X*FTnR6OX0they make it less possible for these groups to offer and experience security in their own families. 心理学空间\Um qm}5u%W
Valuing of attachment relations thus has public policy and moral implications for society, not just
9m3yhSw P(p0psychological implications for attachment dyads. This brings me hack to one of Bowlby’s early
0ZjO2Jvn;q?wcpW~0statements: 心理学空间8kF/[/W(xz V
心理学空间#]DO Sv%q7n``*W
“If a community values its children it must cherish their parents” (Bowlby, 1951, p. 84).
VLFGo'?0y0心理学空间B w#Xi%o(Tv(Y

www.psychspace.com心理学空间网

«玛丽·爱因斯沃斯的陌生情境测验 安思沃斯 Ainsworth
《安思沃斯 Ainsworth》
没有了»